Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-ws8qp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-29T05:31:47.766Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Choline status and neurodevelopmental outcomes at 5 years of age in the Seychelles Child Development Nutrition Study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 January 2013

J. J. Strain*
Affiliation:
Northern Ireland Centre for Food and Health (NICHE), School of Biomedical Sciences, University of Ulster, Coleraine, Northern Ireland, BT52 1SA, UK
Emeir M. McSorley
Affiliation:
Northern Ireland Centre for Food and Health (NICHE), School of Biomedical Sciences, University of Ulster, Coleraine, Northern Ireland, BT52 1SA, UK
Edwin van Wijngaarden
Affiliation:
School of Medicine and Dentistry, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA
Roni W. Kobrosly
Affiliation:
School of Medicine and Dentistry, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA
Maxine P. Bonham
Affiliation:
Department of Nutrition and Dietetics, Monash University, Victoria, Australia
Maria S. Mulhern
Affiliation:
Northern Ireland Centre for Food and Health (NICHE), School of Biomedical Sciences, University of Ulster, Coleraine, Northern Ireland, BT52 1SA, UK
Alison J. McAfee
Affiliation:
Northern Ireland Centre for Food and Health (NICHE), School of Biomedical Sciences, University of Ulster, Coleraine, Northern Ireland, BT52 1SA, UK
Philip W. Davidson
Affiliation:
School of Medicine and Dentistry, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA
Conrad F. Shamlaye
Affiliation:
Child Development Centre, Ministry of Health, Mahé, Republic of Seychelles
Juliette Henderson
Affiliation:
Child Development Centre, Ministry of Health, Mahé, Republic of Seychelles
Gene E. Watson
Affiliation:
School of Medicine and Dentistry, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA
Sally W. Thurston
Affiliation:
School of Medicine and Dentistry, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA
Julie M. W. Wallace
Affiliation:
Northern Ireland Centre for Food and Health (NICHE), School of Biomedical Sciences, University of Ulster, Coleraine, Northern Ireland, BT52 1SA, UK
Per M. Ueland
Affiliation:
Section for Pharmacology, Institute of Medicine, University of Bergen, Haukeland University Hospital, Norway
Gary J. Myers
Affiliation:
School of Medicine and Dentistry, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA
*
*Corresponding author: Professor J. J. Strain, fax +44 28 7012 3023, email jj.strain@ulster.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Choline is an essential nutrient that is found in many food sources and plays a critical role in the development of the central nervous system. Animal studies have shown that choline status pre- and postnatally can have long-lasting effects on attention and memory; however, effects in human subjects have not been well studied. The aim of the present study was to examine the association between plasma concentrations of free choline and its related metabolites in children and their neurodevelopment in the Seychelles Child Development Nutrition Study, an ongoing longitudinal study assessing the development of children born to mothers with high fish consumption during pregnancy. Plasma concentrations of free choline, betaine, dimethylglycine (DMG), methionine and homocysteine and specific measures of neurodevelopment were measured in 210 children aged 5 years. The children's plasma free choline concentration (9·17 (sd 2·09) μmol/l) was moderately, but significantly, correlated with betaine (r 0·24; P= 0·0006), DMG (r 0·15; P= 0·03), methionine (r 0·24; P= 0·0005) and homocysteine (r 0·19; P= 0·006) concentrations. Adjusted multiple linear regression revealed that betaine concentrations were positively associated with Preschool Language Scale – total language scores (β = 0·066; P= 0·04), but no other associations were evident. We found no indication that free choline concentration or its metabolites, within the normal physiological range, are associated with neurodevelopmental outcomes in children at 5 years of age. As there is considerable animal evidence suggesting that choline status during development is associated with cognitive outcome, the issue deserves further study in other cohorts.

Type
Full Papers
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2013 

Choline functions in several important structural and cell signalling roles, which are integral in the formation of VLDL, phospholipids (phosphatidylcholine and sphingomyelin) and the neurotransmitter acetylcholine (ACh)(Reference Ueland1). In addition, choline functions as a methyl donor and is crucial for DNA regulation and repair, protein function and intermediary metabolism. Following cellular uptake, choline is phosphorylated to phosphocholine, or irreversibly oxidised to betaine, which functions to donate methyl groups to homocysteine, producing the essential amino acid methionine(Reference Zeisel, Mar and Howe2). Choline can be synthesised endogenously by methylation of phosphatidylethanolamine, a process which occurs primarily in the liver(Reference Ridgway, Yao and Vance3), but also occurs in neuronal cells(Reference Blusztajn, Liscovitch and Richardson4). However, de novo synthesis alone is not sufficient to meet human requirements(Reference Zeisel and da Costa5). Choline is found naturally in a wide range of foods in the free and esterified form and betaine is also available directly from the diet(6).

A large body of evidence from animal studies suggests that choline supplementation during development improves cognitive and neurological function in offspring(Reference McCann, Hudes and Ames7). Evidence from animal feeding studies suggests a role for choline in hippocampal changes during brain development(Reference Albright, Mar and Friedrich8Reference Wong-Goodrich, Glenn and Mellott10). The hippocampus is critical for the development and consolidation of memory, which along with attention, reasoning, language, perception and construction is a crucial component of human intelligence(Reference Baron11). In human subjects, substantial brain development occurs prenatally and continues to be rapid during the first few years of life. Brain development includes neurogenesis, axonal and dendritic growth, synaptogenesis, cell death, synaptic pruning, myelination and gliogenesis(Reference Grantham-McGregor, Cheung and Cueto12). A number of animal experiments have reported cognitive or other neurological benefits of dietary choline provided either at critical prenatal windows or throughout pregnancy to weaning (for a review, see McCann et al. (Reference McCann, Hudes and Ames7)). Animal studies have also manipulated intake postnatally and reported beneficial effects in the offspring(Reference Meck, Williams and Cermak13Reference Ward, Kolodny and Nag15). In addition, these studies indicate that in animals the effects of choline supplementation or deficiency during development persist to later in life(Reference McCann, Hudes and Ames7, Reference Meck, Williams and Cermak13, Reference Zeisel16).

Despite the widely accepted importance of choline in development of the nervous system, the possible role of choline status on neurodevelopment in children has been investigated in few human studies to date. Signore et al.(Reference Signore, Ueland and Troendle17) prospectively studied 400 mother–child pairs recruited in Birmingham, Alabama, and reported no association at the age of 5 years between cord blood choline concentration and intelligence quotient scores measured using the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Revised. However, the present study reported scores well below the national norms on the developmental tests among both mothers and their children, and thereby raised questions about the study's ability to identify beneficial effects of individual nutrients, such as choline, in the face of poor overall nutrition or other environmental variables that might have contributed to the low scores. Wu et al. (Reference Wu, Dyer and King18) found that maternal choline status in the first half of pregnancy was significantly associated with cognitive development among healthy term gestation infants. To date, no study has examined the impact of the child's, rather than the mother's, choline status (and other biomarkers of one-carbon metabolism) on neurodevelopment.

As the brain continues to develop rapidly during childhood, nutrition is likely to continue to have an impact on neurodevelopment in the preschool years(Reference Rosales, Reznick and Zeisel19). Plasma free choline, for example, is a precursor of the endogenous neurotransmitter ACh and disturbances of the ACh system may impair hippocampus-related cognitive and emotional function(Reference Busche, Bagorda and Lehmann20). Also, epigenetic mechanisms, including DNA methylation, function in the neurobiology of cognition(Reference Sweatt21, Reference Roth, Roth and Sweatt22) and provide plausible mechanisms for a role for choline in development. We postulated that choline exposure in early life could affect hippocampal development, thereby affecting memory and intelligence in children. Therefore, we examined the relationship between biological markers of choline status (and other biomarkers of one-carbon metabolism) and neurodevelopment at 5 years of age in the Seychelles Child Development Nutrition Study.

Subjects and methods

Study population

Participants were recruited from an ongoing study investigating maternal exposure to methylmercury through fish consumption and developmental outcomes in the Republic of Seychelles, an archipelago of over 100 islands in the Indian Ocean, about 1500 km off the coast of East Africa. In 2001, we recruited, at their first antenatal visit (gestational age range 14–24 weeks), 300 healthy pregnant women on the island of Mahé to participate in the study. Of those initially recruited, 256 children completed the 5-year evaluation and complete data on choline variables; endpoints and a priori selected covariates were available for 210 children (106 boys and 104 girls).

The present study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki, and all procedures involving human participants were approved by the Institutional Review Boards of the University of Rochester and the Ministry of Health in the Republic of Seychelles. Written informed consent was obtained from all the participants.

Demographic anthropometric and neurodevelopmental assessments

Birth outcome data, such as birth weight, were obtained from hospital records. When the children were nearing the age 5 years, they were recalled for an evaluation. At the evaluation, mothers completed a questionnaire providing demographic data and the Child Behaviour Checklist (total T-score)(23). The children completed a test battery, which included anthropometry and a series of cognitive, motor and language tests. Children's height and weight were measured by trained nurses and used to calculate BMI z-score based on WHO cut-offs(Reference de Onis, Onyango and Borghi24). All measuring equipment was calibrated prior to initiation of the study, and regularly throughout the study, by the Seychelles Bureau of Standards.

The neurodevelopmental test battery included the following: Finger Tapping (total taps, dominant and non-dominant hand), the Preschool Language Scale (PLS)-Revised (total language, auditory comprehension and verbal knowledge)(Reference Zimmerman, Steiner and Pond25), the Woodcock–Johnson Test of Scholastic Achievement (applied problems, letter-word recognition)(Reference Woodcock and Johnson26), The Child Behaviour Checklist (total T-score)(23) and two of three subtests of the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (verbal reasoning, matrices)(Reference Kaufman and Kaufman27). Testing was accomplished by J. H., a Maternal and Child Health Nurse specially trained at the University of Rochester to administer the test battery. All tests were translated to Creole, the language spoken at home in the Republic of Seychelles by the Kreole Institute in Seychelles. All tests were translated from English to Creole and then back-translated. Pilot testing was conducted to verify that test results were in line with the test norms.

Blood sampling and biochemical measurements

Children's venous non-fasting blood samples were collected in EDTA-containing tubes for plasma separation after completion of the 5-year developmental assessment. Samples were stored in the dark at 4°C until separation, which was performed within 0·5–2·5 h of the time of sampling. Plasma and whole blood samples were stored (for a maximum of 1 year) at − 70°C until batch analysis at the end of the study. Plasma free choline, betaine, dimethylglycine (DMG), total homocysteine (tHcy) and methionine were analysed at the laboratory of Bevital AS (http://www.bevital.no) by liquid chromatography-tandem MS (liquid chromatography-MS/MS), as described previously(Reference Holm, Ueland and Kvalheim28).

Covariates

Analyses controlled for the following covariates known to be associated with child development(Reference Davidson, Myers and Cox29): birth weight (continuous), child's age at testing (continuous), socio-economic status (the Hollingshead Four-Factor Socioeconomic Status modified for use in the Seychelles; continuous), home environment (the Paediatric Review of Children's Environmental Support and Stimulation continuous), maternal intelligence (measured on the Matrices subtest of the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test at the child's 19-month evaluation; continuous), sex of the child, the number of nuclear family members living with the child at the time of the 5-year evaluation (living with both parents v. other situations) and maternal age (continuous).

Data analysis

Bivariate relationships among choline, tHcy and related measures and between these measures and neurodevelopmental endpoints were evaluated using correlation coefficients (Pearson or Spearman depending on variable distribution) and scatter plots. χ2 Tests (for categorical variables) and t tests (for continuous variables) were performed to compare covariate values among the 210 subjects with complete data with those of excluded subjects (for whom some covariate data were not available). Analyses were conducted using R 2.12.0 (R Foundation for Statistical Computing) and SAS version 9.2 (SAS Institute, Inc.). All statistical tests were evaluated using a two-sided P= 0·05 significance level. Non-parametric statistical tests (including the Mann–Whitney test) were used where data could not be normalised (plasma DMG and methionine concentrations) by log-transformation and analysed using parametric tests.

The association of each of the five choline-related measures with each of the ten neurodevelopmental endpoints was estimated using separate multiple linear regression analyses, adjusted for all covariates outlined earlier. As choline metabolism may vary by sex(Reference Chew, Jiang and Yan30), interaction terms between each of the choline measures and sex were included in the models. If the interaction term was not statistically significant, it was excluded and the analysis was rerun. Within each model, we used a two-tailed α-level of 0·05 to determine the significance of interactions and independent variable effects.

Regression assumptions were checked for each model. For models in which the assumption of normally distributed errors with constant variance was violated, we log-transformed the dependent variable to stabilise the variance and produce more normally distributed errors. Variance inflation factors were used as a check for collinearity among variables(Reference Weisberg31). Statistical outliers (defined as observations with standardised residuals greater than 3 in absolute value) and influential points (defined as observations with a Cook's distance larger than 0·50)(Reference Weisberg31) were identified for each model and affected models were then run with and without these values. When results differed substantially with the inclusion or exclusion of outliers or influential points, the differences were noted. Although multiple measures of exposures and cognitive outcomes are included, statistical models are not independent and address the same hypothesis and the interpretation of results is guided by prior data reported in previous cohorts(Reference Strain, Davidson and Thurston32). The present study, therefore, does not correct for multiple testing, which is overly conservative and suffers from numerous limitations(Reference Glantz33, Reference Perneger34).

Results

Relative to the 210 subjects with complete data, excluded subjects (owing to incomplete covariate data, n 46) did not differ significantly in terms of the child's age, sex or birth weight; the mother's age, intelligence or socio-economic status; or the family's home environment. The child's development was assessed at a mean age of 5·61 (sd 0·3) years. In total, 50 % of the subjects were boys. The mean BMI of children at time of testing was 15 (sd 2·1) kg/m2. Characteristics of the study cohort, including concentrations of free choline, betaine, DMG and tHcy, are summarised in Table 1. The concentration of free choline and its related metabolites did not differ significantly between boys and girls. However, sex differences were evident in a number of neurodevelopmental endpoints. Boys performed better than girls in Finger Tapping (total, dominant and non-dominant), while girls performed better than boys in the PLS (total language and verbal knowledge) (Table 1). Free choline concentration was significantly correlated with concentration of betaine (r 0·24; P= 0·0006), DMG (r 0·15; P= 0·03), methionine (r 0·24; P= 0·0005) and tHcy (r 0·19; P= 0·006). Betaine concentration was positively correlated with the logarithm of DMG concentration (r 0·28; P< 0·0001) and negatively correlated with tHcy concentration (r − 0·18; P= 0·01). DMG concentration was positively correlated with methionine concentration (r 0·30; P< 0·001).

Table 1 Summary statistics for predictors, cognitive endpoints and covariates in 5 year-old Seychellois children (Mean values and standard deviations)

PLS, Preschool Language Scale; WJ, Woodcock–Johnson; CBCL, Child Behaviour Checklist; K-BIT, Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test; PROCESS, Paediatric Review of Children's Environmental Support and Stimulation.

* Comparison between boys and girls using t test or Mann–Whitney test as appropriate.

The results of multiple linear regression analyses examining the association between choline measures and neurodevelopmental endpoints are presented in Table 2. There was no interaction between the choline measures and sex in any model. The PLS-total language score improved with increasing plasma betaine concentration (i.e. 0·7-point increase in PLS per standard deviation of betaine concentration (‘standard deviation of betaine’ multiplied by ‘β value for PLS-total language score’)), but there were no other associations present among choline, DMG, methionine or tHcy concentration and any of the endpoints. Results were similar when choline measures were log-transformed.

Table 2 Associations between choline and its related metabolites and neurodevelopmental endpoints in 5 year-old Seychellois children (β Coefficients and their standard errors from adjusted* multiple linear regression analyses)

PLS, Preschool Language Scale; WJ, Woodcock–Johnson; CBCL, Child Behaviour Checklist; K-BIT, Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test.

* Adjusted for birth weight, child's age at testing, socio-economic status, home environment, maternal intelligence, sex of the child, the number of nuclear family members living with the child and maternal age.

For all models, n 210 and df = 200.

Significant association (P= 0·04).

Discussion

The major finding from the present study is the lack of significant associations in covariate-adjusted models between choline measures (and other biomarkers of one-carbon metabolism) and various cognitive outcomes in the 5-year-old children in the Seychelles Child Development Nutrition Cohort. We did not find a significant association between the plasma concentration of free choline, or its related metabolites, and children's intelligence assessed by the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test, a test which measures both verbal and non-verbal intelligence and provides an indication of visual perception, cognitive ability and receptive vocabulary. The Woodcock–Johnson Test of Achievement, which assesses the overall level of scholastic achievement, was also non-significantly associated with choline concentration. In addition, we included measures of neurodevelopment that may be more specifically related to choline. For example, the Finger Tapping Test, which provides a measure of fine motor speed, may be relevant in light of reports that postnatal dietary choline supplementation improves gross motor locomotion in mice(Reference Nag and Berger-Sweeney35). The PLS and Child Behaviour Checklist provide a more specific assessment of visuospatial and auditory memory and social or emotional responses, domains which may be particularly sensitive to choline(Reference Meck, Smith and Williams36, Reference Cheng, MacDonald and Williams37), potentially owing to the influence of ACh on hippocampal plasticity(Reference Busche, Bagorda and Lehmann20). Nevertheless, we did not find any consistent relationships between the endpoints measured and the plasma concentration of choline, DMG, methionine or tHcy. However, plasma betaine concentration was positively associated with the PLS-total language score. A previous intervention study in healthy elderly adults reported a positive effect of betaine on memory performance, which the authors speculated was explained by the greater availability of choline metabolites for synthesis of ACh and structural phospholipids, such as phosphatidylcholine and sphingomyelin(Reference Eussen, Ueland and Clarke38). Such a mechanism is also plausible in children, particularly as the early years are a time of rapid brain growth, with the peak time for development between 5 months gestation and 4 years of age(Reference Shen, Wu and Lin39). Albeit scientifically plausible, the present finding that the PLS-total language score was significantly associated with betaine requires confirmation in future research, as it may be owing to chance.

To our knowledge, the present study is the first to examine the relationship between the plasma concentration of choline and other biomarkers of one-carbon metabolism and concomitant cognitive performance in children. Animal studies, however, have consistently shown a cause-and-effect relationship between postnatal choline intake (and other biomarkers of one-carbon metabolism) and subsequent performance in offspring(Reference McCann, Hudes and Ames7), with beneficial effects reported to be long term and evident in adulthood and old age(Reference Meck, Williams and Cermak13). The majority of research investigating choline intake and performance has been undertaken in animal models, specifically in rodents(Reference McCann, Hudes and Ames7). The growth and development of the rat and human brain progress at different rates, making direct extrapolation of animal data to human subjects difficult. However, similar to the rat, it has been reported that a large proportion (80 %) of the human brain growth spurt is postnatal(Reference Dobbing and Sands40). This observation, together with the evidence that neural plasticity in response to learning continues throughout childhood, would suggest that requirements for brain-related nutrients, including choline, would be important in early childhood(Reference Rosales, Reznick and Zeisel19). The essentiality of nutrients will be related to the timing of their delivery, compared with the critical periods during brain development. However, unlike the prenatal period, the windows of exposure for brain development during the pre-school years are relatively broad(Reference Rosales and Zeisel41), while, furthermore, the capacity for plasticity within the human brain may allow it to compensate for fluctuations in nutritional status.

Choline circulates in a bound form, mostly in the form of phosphatidylcholine, or in a free form initially reported as a likely mechanism supplying choline to the brain(Reference Klein, Koppen and Loffelholz42). Although an accepted and often used biomarker of choline status, plasma free choline represents only a fraction of the total choline pool(Reference Zeisel43) and may be a poor marker of ACh synthesis and status in the brain(Reference Amenta and Tayebati44). Indeed, another study that reported a positive association between betaine status and memory also observed no association with plasma free choline concentration(Reference Eussen, Ueland and Clarke38). Our lack of association may also be attributed to the high plasma choline concentration (status) of the children in the Seychelles. A previous study only observed a relationship between plasma choline concentration and leucocyte ACh concentration in children who had low choline status(Reference Innis, Davidson and Bay45); no relationship was evident in children who had plasma concentrations of choline, betaine and DMG, similar to those reported in the present analysis. The lack of significant associations may also be owing to the small sample size of the present study.

The present study has a number of strengths. The cohort of 300 mothers initially recruited to the present study represented one-fifth of total annual deliveries in the Seychelles and 75 % of all women booking at antenatal clinics during the enrolment period, and was therefore considered to be a representative sample of the population(Reference Bonham, Duffy and Robson46). In addition, the children were sampled and tested early in childhood within a period of significant brain development, and the test battery used specific and reproducible measures of pre-school neurological function. Extensive data on additional factors that influence child development were also collected and controlled for in the present analysis. Furthermore, choline, betaine, DMG and tHcy were assessed using a sensitive method based on liquid chromatography-MS/MS(Reference Holm, Ueland and Kvalheim28), and plasma free choline concentrations were in keeping with values reported previously in US children(Reference Signore, Ueland and Troendle17, Reference Innis, Davidson and Bay45). However, the study also has limitations. The cohort size was not adequate to examine interactions (between choline and the other biomarkers of one-carbon metabolism on neurodevelopmental endpoints) and other unmeasured covariates, such as folate and vitamin B12, which may have been significant. In addition, the critical period, if any, for human postnatal development with respect to choline may not correspond to 5 years of age. As non-fasting samples were collected from the children, plasma choline concentrations may have been sensitive to recent dietary intakes.

In summary, we found few significant associations between the concentration of free choline (within the normal physiological range for children), or its metabolites, and cognitive outcome in children at 5 years of age. However, the experimental evidence suggesting that choline status is positively associated with cognitive outcome in animals supplemented with choline in the postnatal period suggests that this issue needs further investigation.

Acknowledgements

We thank all the women and children who participated in the study and the nursing staff in the Seychelles for their assistance with data collection. The present study was supported by the US National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences, National Institutes of Health (R01-ES010219, R01-ES015578, P30-ES001247 and T32-ES007271), the European Union (Sixth Framework Programme; PHIME; FOOD-CT-2006-016253) and by the Government of Seychelles. The contents reflect only the authors' views; the European Union is not liable for any use that may be made of the information. The authors' responsibilities were as follows: J. M. W. W., M. P. B., E. M. Mc. S. and J. J. S. were involved in the hypothesis generation, organisation of the study, statistical analysis, and data interpretation; E. v. W., R. W. K. and S. W. T. assisted in the statistical analysis and data interpretation; P. W. D., G. J. M., G. E. W. and C. F. S. contributed to the study design and implementation; M. S. M., A. J. Mc. A. and J. H. were involved in the sample analysis and data interpretation; and P. M. U. contributed to the sample analysis; all authors contributed to the manuscript preparation. No author had any conflict of interest.

References

1Ueland, PM (2011) Choline and betaine in health and disease. J Inherit Metab Dis 34, 315.Google Scholar
2Zeisel, SH, Mar, MH, Howe, JC, et al. (2003) Concentrations of choline-containing compounds and betaine in common foods. J Nutr 133, 13021307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3Ridgway, ND, Yao, Z & Vance, DE (1989) Phosphatidylethanolamine levels and regulation of phosphatidylethanolamine N-methyltransferase. J Biol Chem 264, 12031207.Google Scholar
4Blusztajn, JK, Liscovitch, M & Richardson, UI (1987) Synthesis of acetylcholine from choline derived from phosphatidylcholine in a human neuronal cell line. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 84, 54745477.Google Scholar
5Zeisel, SH & da Costa, KA (2009) Choline: an essential nutrient for public health. Nutr Rev 67, 615623.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6USDA (2008) US Department of Agriculture Database for the Choline Content of Common Foods, Release Two. Beltsville, MD: US Department of Agriculture.Google Scholar
7McCann, JC, Hudes, M & Ames, BN (2006) An overview of evidence for a causal relationship between dietary availability of choline during development and cognitive function in offspring. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 30, 696712.Google Scholar
8Albright, CD, Mar, MH, Friedrich, CB, et al. (2001) Maternal choline availability alters the localization of p15Ink4B and p27Kip1 cyclin-dependent kinase inhibitors in the developing fetal rat brain hippocampus. Dev Neurosci 23, 100106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9Albright, CD, Tsai, AY, Mar, MH, et al. (1998) Choline availability modulates the expression of TGFbeta1 and cytoskeletal proteins in the hippocampus of developing rat brain. Neurochem Res 23, 751758.Google Scholar
10Wong-Goodrich, SJ, Glenn, MJ, Mellott, TJ, et al. (2008) Spatial memory and hippocampal plasticity are differentially sensitive to the availability of choline in adulthood as a function of choline supply in utero. Brain Res 1237, 153166.Google Scholar
11Baron, I (2004) Neuropsychological Evaluation of the Child. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
12Grantham-McGregor, S, Cheung, YB, Cueto, S, et al. (2007) Developmental potential in the first 5 years for children in developing countries. Lancet 369, 6070.Google Scholar
13Meck, WH, Williams, CL, Cermak, JM, et al. (2007) Developmental periods of choline sensitivity provide an ontogenetic mechanism for regulating memory capacity and age-related dementia. Front Integr Neurosci 1, 7.Google ScholarPubMed
14Ryan, SH, Williams, JK & Thomas, JD (2008) Choline supplementation attenuates learning deficits associated with neonatal alcohol exposure in the rat: effects of varying the timing of choline administration. Brain Res 1237, 91100.Google Scholar
15Ward, BC, Kolodny, NH, Nag, N, et al. (2009) Neurochemical changes in a mouse model of Rett syndrome: changes over time and in response to perinatal choline nutritional supplementation. J Neurochem 108, 361371.Google Scholar
16Zeisel, SH (2006) The fetal origins of memory: the role of dietary choline in optimal brain development. J Pediatr 149, S131S136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17Signore, C, Ueland, PM, Troendle, J, et al. (2008) Choline concentrations in human maternal and cord blood and intelligence at 5 y of age. Am J Clin Nutr 87, 896902.Google Scholar
18Wu, BTF, Dyer, RA, King, DJ, et al. (2012) Early second trimester maternal plasma choline and betaine are related to measures of early cognitive development in term infants. PLoS One 7, e43448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19Rosales, FJ, Reznick, JS & Zeisel, SH (2009) Understanding the role of nutrition in the brain and behavioral development of toddlers and preschool children: identifying and addressing methodological barriers. Nutr Neurosci 12, 190202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20Busche, A, Bagorda, A, Lehmann, K, et al. (2006) The maturation of the acetylcholine system in the dentate gyrus of gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus) is affected by epigenetic factors. J Neural Transm 113, 113124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21Sweatt, JD (2010) Neuroscience. Epigenetics and cognitive aging. Science 328, 701702.Google Scholar
22Roth, TL, Roth, ED & Sweatt, JD (2010) Epigenetic regulation of genes in learning and memory. Essays Biochem 48, 263274.Google ScholarPubMed
23Achenbach TM (1991) Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist and 1991 Child Behavior Profile. Burlington: Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont.Google Scholar
24de Onis, M, Onyango, AW, Borghi, E, et al. (2007) Development of a WHO growth reference for school-aged children and adolescents. Bull World Health Organ 85, 660667.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25Zimmerman, I, Steiner, V & Pond, RE (1979) Preschool Language Scale. Columbus, OH: Charles Merrill.Google Scholar
26Woodcock, RW & Johnson, MB (1989) Woodcock–Johnson Psycho-educational Battery – Revised. Allen, TX: DLM Teaching Resources.Google Scholar
27Kaufman, AS & Kaufman, NL (2004) Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (KBIT-2), 2nd ed.Bloomington, MN: Pearson, Inc.Google Scholar
28Holm, PI, Ueland, PM, Kvalheim, G, et al. (2003) Determination of choline, betaine, and dimethylglycine in plasma by a high-throughput method based on normal-phase chromatography-tandem mass spectrometry. Clin Chem 49, 286294.Google Scholar
29Davidson, PW, Myers, GJ, Cox, C, et al. (1998) Effects of prenatal and postnatal methylmercury exposure from fish consumption on neurodevelopment: outcomes at 66 months of age in the Seychelles Child Development Study. JAMA 280, 701707.Google Scholar
30Chew, TW, Jiang, X, Yan, J, et al. (2011) Folate intake, MTHFR genotype, and sex modulate choline metabolism in mice. J Nutr 141, 14751481.Google Scholar
31Weisberg, S (2005) Applied Linear Regression. New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
32Strain, JJ, Davidson, PW, Thurston, SW, et al. (2012) Maternal PUFA status but not prenatal methylmercury exposure is associated with children's language functions at age five years in the Seychelles. J Nutr 142, 19431949.Google Scholar
33Glantz, SA (2002) A Primer of Biostatistics. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
34Perneger, TV (1998) What's wrong with Bonferroni adjustments? BMJ 316, 12361238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
35Nag, N & Berger-Sweeney, JE (2007) Postnatal dietary choline supplementation alters behavior in a mouse model of Rett syndrome. Neurobiol Dis 26, 473480.Google Scholar
36Meck, WH, Smith, RA & Williams, CL (1988) Pre- and postnatal choline supplementation produces long-term facilitation of spatial memory. Dev Psychobiol 21, 339353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
37Cheng, RK, MacDonald, CJ, Williams, CL, et al. (2008) Prenatal choline supplementation alters the timing, emotion, and memory performance (TEMP) of adult male and female rats as indexed by differential reinforcement of low-rate schedule behavior. Learn Mem 15, 153162.Google Scholar
38Eussen, SJ, Ueland, PM, Clarke, R, et al. (2007) The association of betaine, homocysteine and related metabolites with cognitive function in Dutch elderly people. Br J Nutr 98, 960968.Google Scholar
39Shen, EY, Wu, KH, Lin, MF, et al. (2010) Study of brain growth in children – a new approach to volume measurements using MRI-reconstructed 3D neuroimaging. Childs Nerv Syst 26, 16191623.Google Scholar
40Dobbing, J & Sands, J (1973) Quantitative growth and development of human brain. Arch Dis Child 48, 757767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
41Rosales, FJ & Zeisel, SH (2008) Perspectives from the symposium: the role of nutrition in infant and toddler brain and behavioral development. Nutr Neurosci 11, 135143.Google Scholar
42Klein, J, Koppen, A, Loffelholz, K, et al. (1992) Uptake and metabolism of choline by rat brain after acute choline administration. J Neurochem 58, 870876.Google Scholar
43Zeisel, SH (2004) Nutritional importance of choline for brain development. J Am Coll Nutr 23, 621S626S.Google Scholar
44Amenta, F & Tayebati, SK (2008) Pathways of acetylcholine synthesis, transport and release as targets for treatment of adult-onset cognitive dysfunction. Curr Med Chem 15, 488498.Google Scholar
45Innis, SM, Davidson, AG, Bay, BN, et al. (2011) Plasma choline depletion is associated with decreased peripheral blood leukocyte acetylcholine in children with cystic fibrosis. Am J Clin Nutr 93, 564568.Google Scholar
46Bonham, MP, Duffy, EM, Robson, PJ, et al. (2009) Contribution of fish to intakes of micronutrients important for fetal development: a dietary survey of pregnant women in the Republic of Seychelles. Public Health Nutr 12, 13121320.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Summary statistics for predictors, cognitive endpoints and covariates in 5 year-old Seychellois children (Mean values and standard deviations)

Figure 1

Table 2 Associations between choline and its related metabolites and neurodevelopmental endpoints in 5 year-old Seychellois children (β Coefficients and their standard errors from adjusted* multiple linear regression analyses)†