Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-mp689 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-18T14:09:42.569Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Causal links, contingencies, and the comparative psychology of intelligence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 May 2011

Juan C. Gómez
Affiliation:
Departamento de Psicologia Evolutiva, Facultad de Psicologia, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Madrid 28049, Spain

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Continuing Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1990

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, R., Maurer, D. & Davis, M. (1986) Newborns’ discrimination of chromatic from achromatic stimuli. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 41:267–81. [SFW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atkinson, J. (1984) Human visual development over the first six months of life: A review and a hypothesis. Human Neurobiology 3:6174. [SFW]Google Scholar
Beggs, A. L., Steinmetz, J. E. & Patterson, M. M. (1985) Classical conditioning of a flexor nerve response in spinal cats: Effects of tibial nerve CS and a differential conditioning paradigm. Behavioral Neuroscience 99:496508. [rEMM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beggs, A. L., Steinmetz, J. E., Romano, A. G. & Patterson, M. M. (1983) Extinction and retention of a classically conditioned flexor nerve response in acute spinal cat. Behavioral Neuroscience 97:530–40. [rEMM, SFW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Birch, H. G. (1945) The relation of previous experience to insightful problem solving. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 38:367–83. [JCG]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carey, S. (1985) Are children fundamentally different kinds of thinkers and learners than adults? In: Thinking and learning skills, vol. 2: Research and open questions ed. Chipman, S. F., Segal, J. W., & Glaser, R., Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [MR]Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1972) Language and mind, enlarged edition. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. [rEMM]Google Scholar
Clark, A. (1987) The kludge in the machine. Mind and Language 2:277300. [SFW]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickinson, A. (1985) Actions and habits: The development of behavioural autonomy. Proceedings of the Royal Society, B 308:6778. [SFW]Google Scholar
Doré, F. Y., & Dumas, C. (1987) Psychology of animal cognition: Piagetian studies. Psychological Bulletin 102:219–33. [rEMM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doré, F. Y. & Kirouac, G. (1987) What comparative psychology is about: Back to the future. Journal of Comparative Psychology 101:242–48. [rEMM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doré, F. Y., & Dumas, C. (1987) Psychology of animal cognition: Piagetian studies. Psychological Bulletin 102:219–33. [JCG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Etienne, A. S. (1984) The meaning of object permanence at different zoological levels. Human Development 27:309–20. [JCG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Furnham, A. F. (1988) Lay theories: Everyday understanding of problems in the social sciences. Pergamon Press. [JCG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, E. J. (1987) Introductory essay: What does infant perception tell us about theories of perception? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 13:515–23. [MR]Google Scholar
Goldman-Rakic, P. S., & Preuss, T. M. (1987) “Wither” comparative psychology? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10(4):666–67. [MR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gómez, J. C. (1988) The development of means-ends intelligent coordinations in an infant gorilla. Presented at the Third European Conference on Developmental Psychology, Budapest. [JCG]Google Scholar
Gould, J. L. (1986) The locale map of honey bees: Do insects have cognitive maps? Science 232:861–63. [JV]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffin, D., ed. (1982) Animal mind-human mind. Springer Verlag. [JV]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Groves, P. M. & Thompson, R. F. (1970) Habituation: A dual-process theory. Psychological Review 77:419–50. [SFW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gruber, H. E., Girgus, J. S. & Banuazizi, A. (1971) The development of object permanence in the cat. Developmental Psychology 4:915. [rEMM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harnad, S., ed. (1987) Categorical perception: The groundwork of cognition. Cambridge University Press. [SFW]Google Scholar
Heaton, M. B., Galleher, E. L., Baker, R. T., Otero, J. M. & Alvarez, I. M. (1981) Operant escape learning in decerebrate duck embryos. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 95:199204. [SFW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hulse, S. H., Fowler, H. & Honig, W. K., eds. (1978) Cognitive processes in animal behavior. Lawrence Erlbaum. [JV]Google Scholar
Jerison, H. J. (1973) Evolution of the brain and intelligence. Academic Press. [rEMM, SFW]Google Scholar
Katz, D. (1953) Animals and men. Penguin Books. [rEMM]Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1917) Intelligenzpriifungen an Anthropoiden. Ahhandlungen der Preussische Akademie der Wissenschaften. Physikalische- Mathematische Klasse. Nr. 1. (Second Edition: Intelligenzpriifungen an Menschenaffen, 1921. Springer.) [JCG]Google Scholar
(1925) The mentality of apes. Routledge & Kegan Paul. [rEMM]Google Scholar
Leaton, R. N. & Supple, W. F. (1986) Cerebellar vermis: Essential for long-term habituation of the acoustic startle response. Science 232:513–15. [rEMM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Livingstone, M. S. & Hubel, D. H. (1984) Anatomy and physiology of a colour system in the primate visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience 4:309–56. [SFW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mackintosh, N. J. (1975) A theory of attention: Variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement. Psychological Review 82:276–98. [rEMM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
(1983)Conditioning and associative learning. Clarendon Press. [SFW]Google Scholar
Macphail, E. M. (1982) Brain and intelligence in vertebrates. Clarendon Press. [rEMM, JV, SFW]Google Scholar
(1985a) Vertebrate intelligence: The null hypothesis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B 308:3751. [rEMM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
(1985b) Ecology and intelligence. In: Memory systems of the brain: Animal and human cognitive processes, ed. Weinberger, N. M., McGaugh, J. L. & Lynch, G.. Guilford Press. [rEMM]Google Scholar
(1986) Animal memory: Past, present and future. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 38B:349–64. [rEMM]Google Scholar
(1987) The comparative psychology of intelligence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10:645–96. [JCG, MR, JV, SFW]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miyashita, Y. & Chang, H. S. (1988) Neuronal correlate of pictorial short-term memory in the primate temporal cortex. Nature 331:6970. [SFW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neiworth, J. J. & Rilling, M. E. (1987) A method for studying imagery in animals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 13:203–14. [rEMM, MR]Google Scholar
Oakley, D. A. (1979) Instrumental reversal learning and subsequent fixed ratio performance on simple and go/no-go schedules in neodecorticate rabbits. Physiological Psychology 7:2942. [SFW]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
(1983) The varieties of memory: A phylogenetic approach. In: Memory in animals and humans, ed. Mayes, A.. Van Nostrand. [SFW]Google Scholar
Oakley, D. A., & Russell, I. S. (1976) Subcortical nature of Pavlovian differentiation in the rabbit. Physiology and Behaviour 17:947–54. [SFW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olson, G. M. (1976) An information-processing analysis of visual memory and habituation in infants. In: Habituation, ed. Tighe, T. J. & Leaton, R. N.. Lawrence Erlbaum. [SFW]Google Scholar
Parker, S. T. (1977) Piaget’s sensorimotor series in an infant macaque: A model for comparing unstereotyped behavior and intelligence in human and nonhuman primates. In: Primate biosocial development, ed. Chevalier-Skolnikoff, S. & Poirier, F.. Garland. [JCG]Google Scholar
Patterson, M. M. (1976) Mechanisms of classical conditioning and fixation in spinal mammals. In: Advances in psychobiology, vol. 3, ed. Riesen, A. & Thompson, R.. Wiley. [rEMM]Google Scholar
Pavlov, I. P. (1927) Conditioned reflexes: An investigation of the physiological activity of the cerebral cortex. Dover. [SFW]Google Scholar
Pearce, J. M., & Hall, G. (1980) A model for Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. Psychological Review 87:532–52. [rEMM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1936) La naissance de l'intelligence chez l'enfant. Delachaux et Niestlé. [JCG]Google Scholar
(1937) La construction du reel chez l'enfant. Delachaux et Niestlé. [JCG]Google Scholar
(1947) La psychologie de Fintelligence. A. Colin. [JCG]Google Scholar
Raaheim, K. (1988) Intelligence and task novelty. In: Advance in the psychology of human intelligence, vol. 4. ed. Sternbere, R. J.. Lawrence Erlbaum. [MR]Google Scholar
Rescorla, R. A. (1985) Facilitation and inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 13:250–59. [rEMM]Google Scholar
Rescorla, R. A. & Wagner, A. R. (1972) A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In: Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory, ed. Black, A. H. & Prokasy, W. F.. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [rEMM]Google Scholar
Richards, R. J. (1987) Darwin and the emergence of evolutionary theories of mind and behavior. The University of Chicago Press. [MR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rilling, M. E. & Neiworth, J. J. (1987) Theoretical and methodological considerations for the study of imagery in animals. Learning and Motivation 18:57–79. [MR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roitblat, H. L. (1987) Metacomparative psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10(4):677–78. [MR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roitblat, H. L., Bever, T. G. & Terrace, H. S., eds. (1984) Animal cognition. Lawrence Erlbaum. [JV, SFW]Google Scholar
Scholes, N. W. (1965) Detour learning and development in the domestic chick. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 60:114–16. [rEMM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shepard, R. N. (1984) Ecological constraints on internal representation: Resonant kinematics of perceiving, imagining, thinking, and dreaming. Psychological Review 91:417–47. [MR]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shettleworth, S. J. (1987) Intelligence: More than a matter of associations. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10(4):679. [MR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sokolov, E. N. (1975) The neuronal mechanisms of the orienting reflex. In: Neuronal mechanisms of the orienting reflex, ed. Sokolov, E. N. & Vinogradova, O. S.. Lawrence Erlbaum. [SFW]Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (ed.). (1982) Handbook of human intelligence. Cambridge University Press (Cambridge). [MR]Google Scholar
Thompson, R. F. & Spencer, W. A. (1966) Habituation: A model phenomenon for the study of the neuronal substrates of behaviour. Psychological Review 73:1643. [SFW]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turkkan, J. S. (1989) Classical conditioning: The new hegemony. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12(1): 121179. [MR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vauclair, J. (in press) Primate cognition: From representation to language. In: Language and intelligence in monkeys and apes. Comparative developmental perspectives, ed. Gibson, K. & Parker, S.. Cambridge University Press. [JV]Google Scholar
Walker, S. F. (1985) Animal thought. Routledge & Kegan Paul. [SFW]Google Scholar
(1987a) Animal learning. Routledge & Kegan Paul. [SFW]Google Scholar
(1987b) The evaluation and dissolution of language. In: Progress in the psychology of language, vol. 3, ed. Ellis, A.. Lawrence Erlbaum. [SFW]Google Scholar
Weiskrantz, L. (1988) Thought without language. Oxford University Press (New York). [MR]Google Scholar