Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-wzw2p Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-03T04:54:02.339Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Factors associated with lowercase alphabet naming in kindergarteners

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 December 2012

FRANCIS L. HUANG*
Affiliation:
University of Virginia
MARCIA A. INVERNIZZI
Affiliation:
University of Virginia
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Francis L. Huang, Curry School of Education, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA 22903. E-mail: fhh2b@virginia.edu

Abstract

We investigated five hypotheses related to the successful naming of lowercase letters. Participants included 5,020 first-time kindergarteners from economically disadvantaged homes who previously attended publicly funded preschools. Results analyzed using three-level logistic regression showed that children have a higher probability of correctly identifying letters that appear in their own name and letters that appear frequently in print. In addition, lowercase letter shape similarity to its uppercase counterpart and letter order were also associated with the correct identification of the letter. Finally, if a lowercase letter had a visual and phonological resemblance to other letters, students had a lower probability of identifying the letter correctly. Implications for instruction are discussed.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Adams, M. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Champaign, IL: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Agresti, A., & Finlay, B. (2009). Statistical methods for the social sciences (4th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Arciuli, J., & Simpson, I. C. (2011). Not all letters are created equal: Exploring letter name knowledge through spelling in school children and adults. Writing Systems Research, 3, 113.Google Scholar
Bear, D. R., Invernizzi, M., Templeton, S., & Johnston, F. (2012). Words their way: Word study for phonics, spelling, and vocabulary instruction (5th ed.). New York: Pearson Education.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (1991). Letters, sounds, and symbols: Changes in children's understanding of written language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 7589.Google Scholar
Blair, J., & Ryckman, D. (1969). Visual discrimination: Lowercase letter confusion (Technical Report). Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Bloodgood, J. W. (1999). What's in a name? Children's name writing and literacy acquisition. Reading Research Quarterly, 34, 342367.Google Scholar
Boles, D., & Clifford, J. (1989). An upper- and lowercase alphabetic similarity matrix, with derived generation similarity values. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 21, 579586.Google Scholar
Bracey, G. (1989). Age and achievement. Phi Delta Kappan, 70, 732.Google Scholar
Briggs, R., & Hocevar, D. (1975). A new distinctive feature theory for uppercase letters. Journal of General Psychology, 93, 8793.Google Scholar
Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Tomblin, J. B., & Zhang, X. (2002). A longitudinal investigation of reading outcomes in children with language impairments. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 11421157.Google Scholar
Catts, H. W., Fey, M. E., Zhang, X., & Tomblin, J. B. (2001). Estimating the risk of future reading difficulties in kindergarten children: A research-based model and its clinical implementation. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 32, 3850.Google Scholar
Chinn, S. (2000). A simple method for converting an odds ratio to effect size for use in meta-analysis. Statistics in Medicine, 19, 31273131.Google Scholar
Chiu, M. M., & McBride-Chang, C. (2006). Gender, context, and reading: A comparison of students in 43 countries. Scientific Studies of Reading, 4, 331362.Google Scholar
Clay, M. (1975). What did I write? Auckland, New Zealand: Heinemann.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155159.Google Scholar
Cohn, M. (1974). Letter recognition difficulties: Their real nature. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the International Reading Association, New Orleans.Google Scholar
Cohn, M., & Stricker, G. (1976). Inadequate perception vs. reversals. Reading Teacher, 30, 162167.Google Scholar
Cohn, M., & Stricker, G. (1979). Reversal errors in strong, average, and weak letter names. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 12, 533537.Google Scholar
Crone, D., & Whitehurst, G. (1999). Age and schooling effects on emergent literacy and early reading skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 604614.Google Scholar
Cunningham, A., & Carroll, J. (2011). Age and schooling effects on early literacy and phoneme awareness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109, 248255.Google Scholar
Ehri, L. C. (1983). A critique of five studies related to letter-name knowledge and learning to read. In Gentile, L. M., Kamil, M. L., & Blanchard, J. S. (Eds.), Reading research revisited (pp. 143153). Columbus, OH: Merrill.Google Scholar
Ehri, L., & Roberts, T. (2006). The roots of learning to read and write: Acquisition of letters and phonemic awareness. In Dickinson, D. K. & Neuman, S. B. (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (2nd ed., pp. 113134). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Ellefson, M. R., Treiman, R., & Kessler, B. (2009). Learning to label letters by sounds or names: A comparison of England and the United States. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 323341.Google Scholar
Evans, M., Bell, M., Shaw, D., Moretti, S., & Page, J. (2006). Letter names, letter sounds, and phonological awareness: An examination of kindergarten children across letters and of letters across children. Reading and Writing, 19, 959989.Google Scholar
Eviatar, Z., Ibramim, R., & Ganayim, D. (2004). Orthography and the hemispheres: Visual and linguistic aspects of letter processing. Neuropsychology, 18, 174184.Google Scholar
Fantuzzo, J., Manz, P., & McDermott, P. (1998). Preschool version of the Social Skills Rating System: An empirical analysis of its use with low-income children. Journal of School Psychology, 36, 199214.Google Scholar
Fiset, D., Blais, C., Éthier-Majcher, C., Arguin, M., Bub, D., & Gosselin, F. (2008). Features for identification of uppercase and lowercase letters. Psychological Science, 19, 11611168.Google Scholar
Foulin, J. N. (2005). Why is letter-name knowledge such a good predictor of learning to read? Reading and Writing, 18, 129155.Google Scholar
Gibson, E., Gibson, J., Pick, A., & Osser, H. (1962). A developmental study of the discrimination of letter-like forms. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 55, 897906.Google Scholar
Groff, P. (1972). A new sequence for teaching lowercase letters. Journal of Literacy Research, 5, 297303.Google Scholar
Guo, G., & Zhao, H. (2000). Multilevel modeling for binary data. Annual Review of Sociology, 26, 441462.Google Scholar
Guralnick, M. (1972). Alphabet discrimination and distinctive features: Research review and educational implications. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 5, 428461.Google Scholar
Hammill, D. D. (2004). What we know about correlates of reading. Exceptional Children, 70, 453468.Google Scholar
Haney, M. R. (2002). Name writing: A window into the emergent literacy skills of young children. Early Childhood Education Journal, 30, 101105.Google Scholar
Hildreth, G. (1936). Developmental sequences in name writing. Child Development, 7, 291303.Google Scholar
Ibrahim, R., Eviatar, Z., & Aharon-Peretz, J. (2002). The characteristics of Arabic orthography slow its processing. Neuropsychology, 16, 322326.Google Scholar
Invernizzi, M., Justice, L., Landrum, T. J., & Booker, K. (2004). Early literacy screening in kindergarten: Widespread implementation in Virginia. Journal of Literacy Research, 36, 479500.Google Scholar
Invernizzi, M., Swank, L., Juel, C., & Meier, J. (2003). Phonological Awareness Literacy Screening—Kindergarten. Charlottesville, VA: University Printing.Google Scholar
Jones, M. N., & Mewhort, D. J. K. (2004). Case-sensitive letter and bigram frequency counts from large-scale English corpora. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 36, 388396.Google Scholar
Justice, L. M., Pence, K., Bowles, R. B., & Wiggins, A. (2006). An investigation of four hypotheses concerning the order by which 4-year-old children learn the alphabet letters. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 21, 374389.Google Scholar
Kim, Y., Petscher, Y., Foorman, B. R., & Zhou, C. (2010). The contributions of phonological awareness and letter-name knowledge to letter-sound acquisition—A cross-classified multilevel model approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 313326.Google Scholar
Klein, S., Hamilton, L. S., McCaffrey, D. F., & Stecher, B. M. (2000). What do test scores in Texas tell us? Santa Monica, CA: RAND.Google Scholar
Ladd, G., & Profilet, S. (1996). The child behavior scale: A teacher-report measure of young children's aggressive, withdrawn, and prosocial behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 32, 10081024.Google Scholar
Lehr, F. R. (2000). The sequence of speech–sound acquisition in the Letter People program. Waterbury, CT: Abrams & Company.Google Scholar
Levin, I., & Aram, D. (2004). Children's names contribute to early literacy: A linguistic and social perspective. In Ravid, D. & Shyldkrot, H. (Eds.), Perspectives on language and language development (pp. 219239). Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Levin, I., & Ehri, L. C. (2009). Young children's ability to read and spell their own and classmates’ names: The role of letter knowledge. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13, 249273.Google Scholar
Levin, I., Saiegh-Haddad, E., Hende, N., & Ziv, M. (2008). Early literacy in Arabic: An intervention study among Israeli Palestinian kindergartners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29, 413434.Google Scholar
Mayzner, M. S., & Tresselt, M. E. (1965). Tables of single-letter and diagram frequency counts for various word-length and letter-position combinations. Psychonomic Monograph Supplements, 1, 1332.Google Scholar
McBride-Chang, C. (1999). The ABCs of the ABCs: The development of letter-name and letter-sound knowledge. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly, 45, 285308.Google Scholar
McCardle, P., Scarborough, H. S., & Catts, H. W. (2001). Predicting, explaining, and preventing children's reading difficulties. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 16, 230239.Google Scholar
Morrison, F., & Griffith, E., & Alberts, D. (1997). Nature–nurture in the classroom: Entrance age, school readiness, and learning in children. Developmental Psychology, 33, 254262.Google Scholar
O'Connor, R. E., & Jenkins, J. R. (1999). Prediction of reading disabilities in kindergarten and first grade. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 159197.Google Scholar
Piasta, S. B., Purpura, D. J., & Wagner, R. K. (2010). Fostering alphabet knowledge development: A comparison of two instructional approaches. Reading and Writing, 23, 607626.Google Scholar
Piasta, S. B., & Wagner, R. K. (2010a). Developing early literacy skills: A meta-analysis of alphabet learning and instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 45, 838.Google Scholar
Piasta, S. B., & Wagner, R. K. (2010b). Learning letter names and sounds: Effects of instruction, letter type, and phonological processing skill. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105, 324344.Google Scholar
Popp, H. M. (1964). Visual discrimination of alphabet letters. Reading Teacher, 17, 221226.Google Scholar
Pressley, M., Rankin, J., & Yokoi, L. (1996). A survey of instructional practices of primary teachers nominated as effective in promoting literacy. Elementary School Journal, 96, 363384.Google Scholar
Puranik, C., Lonigan, C., & Kim, Y. (2011). Contributions of emergent literacy skills to name writing, letter writing, and spelling in preschool children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 26, 465474.Google Scholar
Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods (2nd ed.). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Rosenthal, R., & Jacobson, L. (1968). Pygmalion in the classroom. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.Google Scholar
Sander, E. (1972). When are speech sounds learned? Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 37, 5563.Google Scholar
Scarborough, H. S. (1998). Early identification of children at risk for reading disabilities: Phonological awareness and some other promising predictors. In Shapiro, B. K., Accardo, P. J., & Capute, A. J. (Eds.), Specific reading disability: A view of the spectrum (pp. 75119). Timonium, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Schatschneider, C., Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D. J., Carlson, C. D., & Foorman, B. R. (2004). Kindergarten prediction of reading skills: A longitudinal comparative analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 265282.Google Scholar
Share, D. (2004). Knowing letter names and learning letter sounds: A causal connection. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 99, 213233.Google Scholar
Smith, J. (2000). Singing and songwriting support early literacy instruction. Reading Teacher, 53, 646651.Google Scholar
Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Stevenson, H. W., & Newman, R. S. (1986). Long-term prediction of achievement and attitudes in mathematics and reading. Child Development, 57, 646659.Google Scholar
Tajima, A. (2004). Fatal miscommunication: English in aviation safety. World Englishes, 23, 451470.Google Scholar
Thorson, G. (1976). An alternative for judging confusability of visual letters. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 42, 116118.Google Scholar
Treiman, R. (2000). The foundations of literacy. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 8992.Google Scholar
Treiman, R., & Broderick, V. (1998). What's in a name: Children's knowledge about the letters in their own names. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 70, 97116.Google Scholar
Treiman, R., Cohen, J., Mulqueeny, K., Kessler, B., & Schechtman, S. (2007). Young children's knowledge about printed names. Child Development, 78, 14581471.Google Scholar
Treiman, R., & Kessler, B. (2003). The role of letter names in the acquisition of literacy. In Kail, R. V. (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 31, pp. 105135). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Treiman, R., & Kessler, B. (2004). The case of case: Children's knowledge and use of upper- and lowercase letters. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 413428.Google Scholar
Treiman, R., & Kessler, B. (2011). Similarities among the shapes of writing and their effects on learning. Written Language and Literacy, 14, 3957.Google Scholar
Treiman, R., Kessler, B., & Bourassa, D. (2001). Children's own names influence their spelling. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 555570.Google Scholar
Treiman, R., Kessler, B., & Pollo, T. C. (2006). Learning about the letter name subset of the vocabulary: Evidence from US and Brazilian preschoolers. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 211227.Google Scholar
Treiman, R., Levin, I., & Kessler, B. (2007). Learning of letter names follows similar principles across languages: Evidence from Hebrew. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96, 87106.Google Scholar
Treiman, R., Levin, I., & Kessler, B. (2010). Linking the shapes of alphabet letters to their sounds: The case of Hebrew. Reading and Writing. Advance online publication. doi:10.1007/s11145–010-9286–3Google Scholar
Treiman, R., Tincoff, R., Rodriguez, K., Mouzaki, A., & Francis, D. J. (1998). The foundations of literacy: Learning the sounds of letters. Child Development, 69, 15241540.Google Scholar
Turnbull, K. L. P., Bowles, R. P., Skibbe, L. E., Justice, L. M., & Wiggins, A. K. (2010). Theoretical explanations for preschoolers’ lowercase alphabet knowledge. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, 17571768.Google Scholar
Whitehurst, G. J., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Child development and emergent literacy. Child Development, 69, 848872.Google Scholar
Worden, P. E., & Boettcher, W. (1990). Young children's acquisition of alphabet knowledge. Journal of Reading Behavior, 23, 277295.Google Scholar
Wrighton, C. A. (n.d.). Creating brain-efficient curriculum. Retrieved from http://www.zoo-phonics.com/images/research_wrighton.pdfGoogle Scholar
Zeno, S. M., Ivenz, S. H., Millard, R. T., & Duvvuri, R. (1995). The Educator's Word Frequency Guide. Brewster, NY: Touchstone Applied Science Associates.Google Scholar