Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-jr42d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T09:47:34.161Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Exposure to early adversity: Points of cross-species translation that can lead to improved understanding of depression

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 May 2015

Susan L. Andersen*
Affiliation:
Harvard Medical School
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Susan L. Andersen, Laboratory for Developmental, Neuropharmacology, Mailstop 333, McLean Hospital/Harvard Medical School, Belmont, MA 02478; E-mail: sandersen@mclean.harvard.edu.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The relationship between developmental exposure to adversity and affective disorders is reviewed. Adversity discussed herein includes physical and sexual abuse, neglect, or loss of a caregiver in humans. While these stressors can occur at any point during development, the unique temporal relationship to specific depressive symptoms was the focus of discussion. Further influences of stress exposure during sensitive periods can vary by gender and duration of abuse as well. Data from animal studies are presented to provide greater translational and causal understanding of how sensitive periods, different types of psychosocial stressors, and sex interact to produce depressive-like behaviors. Findings from maternal separation, isolation rearing, chronic variable stress, and peer–peer rearing paradigms clarify interpretation about how various depressive behaviors are influenced by age of exposure. Depressive behaviors are broken down into the following categories: mood and affect, anhedonia, energy, working memory, sleep–wake, appetite changes, suicide, and general malaise. Cross-species evidence from humans, nonhuman primates, rats, and mice within each of these categories is discussed. In conclusion, sensitive periods for affective-related behaviors (anxiety, mood, and controllability) occur earlier in life, while other aspects of depression are associated with adversity later during adolescence.

Type
Regular Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Approximately 33% of the population is exposed to adversity during the course of development in the form of physical/sexual abuse, neglect, loss of a parent or caregiver, or a natural disaster. Bullying is another form of adversity to which approximately 25% of children under the age of 11 years are exposed. Depression occurs in nearly 67% of the early life maltreatment population (Andersen & Teicher, Reference Andersen and Teicher2008; Andersen et al., Reference Andersen, Tomada, Vincow, Valente, Polcari and Teicher2008; Widom, DuMont, & Czaja, Reference Widom, DuMont and Czaja2007), compared to a lifetime prevalence rate in the general population of approximately 20% (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, McGonagle, Zhao, Nelson, Hughes and Eshleman1994). Depression emerges earlier than normal in children with a maltreatment history (Teicher, Samson, Polcari, & Andersen, Reference Teicher, Samson, Polcari and Andersen2009; Widom et al., Reference Widom, DuMont and Czaja2007). The Treatment of SSRI-Resistant Depression in Adolescents Study has shown that cases of depression that present with a severe baseline of emotional dysregulation, a sense of hopelessness, elevated anxiety, and high levels of stress due to family conflict are less likely to achieve remission (Brent, Reference Brent2009). The majority of individuals with a history of maltreatment or childhood anxiety disorder fall into this group.

The causal relationship between a history of maltreatment and subsequent depression is difficult to prove in human studies. Prospective studies (e.g., Widom et al., Reference Widom, DuMont and Czaja2007) suggest that individuals with a developmental exposure to adversity have a higher risk of depression. Similarly, co-twin studies where women were exposed to varying degrees of childhood sexual abuse also show increased vulnerability to depression (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Bulik, Silberg, Hettema, Myers and Prescott2000; Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, Heath, Madden, Cooper, Dinwiddie and Bucholz2002). Greater understanding of the relationship between maltreatment and depressive outcomes is likely to have an impact on improved treatment approaches, especially given the limited positive outcome from current pharmacological approaches.

This review paper will examine what is known about the relationship between depression and childhood exposure to maltreatment from a translational viewpoint. Various paradigms of early life stress are presented to address the maturational timing issue, followed by a breakdown of findings reported in the literature of specific symptoms of depression. This review does not include any extensive discussion of the role of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis (HPA) that certainly influences depression; the reader is referred to (Eiland & Romeo, Reference Eiland and Romeo2013) for such a discussion. Finally, ways to improve existing preventative interventions in the treatment of depression in this population are discussed.

Basic Definition and Across-Species Comparisons

The potential for any translational study to have an impact lies in its ability to facilitate valid, reliable, and direct comparisons across species. Behavioral, biochemical, or anatomical changes can provide points of shared reference. This framework is one through which cross-species comparisons are made in this paper with the goal of identifying translational points of commonality. Relationships among development, adversity, and depression will be examined with a particular focus on assessment across human, nonhuman primate, and rodents.

Utility of animal models

Animal model often refers to the use of animals to demonstrate a behavioral or biochemical phenotype that is found in humans. Given that animals will never sufficiently model the human condition and its complexities (Andersen & Thompson, Reference Andersen and Thompson2011), the more appropriate terminology that should be used is endophenotype (e.g., Castellanos & Tannock, Reference Castellanos and Tannock2002). Endophenotypes are elemental components of a given disorder or condition that allows direct comparisons to be made across species if they have the same effect.

From a slightly different perspective, the Research Domain Criteria Project framework provides “constructs” and “domains” that are more amenable to translation. A construct is the basic unit of analysis, which includes motivation, cognition, and social behavior; the NIMH lists five domain systems: negative or positive valence, cognition, social processing, and arousal/regulatory (www.nimh.nih.gov). The next step taken in this categorization accounts for the level of analysis. Recognizing that analyses occur from behavior to genes and span from circuits to physiology, all the information necessary to produce a complete picture can occur neither within clinical studies nor in animal studies. The interdependence among different species is needed to fully understand the domains.

Another point about the use of animals needs to be addressed: evolutionary adaptation. Behaviors evolve over a very long time (tens of thousands of years) to increase the survival of the species but not necessarily the individual; other behaviors are passed across generations (Maestripieri, Reference Maestripieri2005). As environmental demands can shift relatively quickly, the species may be slow to adapt or display adaptive behaviors that do not seem to make sense in the current environment, but may allow the animal to survive a hostile world (Teicher, Reference Teicher2002); this model is known as adaptive calibration (McEwen, Reference McEwen2000). For example, corticosteroids may mediate the maturation of adult defensive systems, such that low levels of stress (e.g., novelty) will down-regulate reactivity to external stressors whereas high levels increase reactivity (Macri, Zoratto, & Laviola, Reference Macri, Zoratto and Laviola2011). Equally worth considering is at what point behavior is “abnormal” (reviewed in (Haller & Kruk, Reference Haller and Kruk2006). As development proceeds, adolescence is characterized by typical, yet more extreme, changes in behavior (e.g., risk taking and moodiness) and adolescence is also the period for the emergence of psychopathology (Cicchetti & Rogosch, Reference Cicchetti and Rogosch2002). Epigenetics provides a mechanism to pass environmental impact across the generations (Champagne, Reference Champagne2013).

A final consideration is how manipulations in animals parallel findings in humans and what that difference can tell us. While we aim to explain the human condition, animals may not experience the same psychosocial pressures or at least we do not know enough to model them. In other words, data from both sides can increase the understanding of the other.

Age-related differences in development in general

The brain continues to mature into the third decade of life. In general, synapses are formed and then competitively pruned back to optimize function for the demands of the environment (Brenhouse & Andersen, Reference Brenhouse and Andersen2011). This process occurs differentially across brain regions. More fundamental brain regions undergo this process earlier, whereas the more complex brain regions, such as the cortex, prune later in adolescence and young adulthood. Different neurotransmitters play different roles during maturation, with GABA and glutamate acting as principal players (discussed below). We have previously reviewed this subject in typically developing species (Brenhouse & Andersen, Reference Brenhouse and Andersen2011).

Sensitive periods of vulnerability

Sensitive periods are points in maturation when environmental input has a maximal impact on development. Sensitive periods differ from critical periods. Critical periods are well described in sensory systems (Hubel & Wiesel, Reference Hubel and Wiesel1970, Reference Hubel and Wiesel1998; Hubel, Wiesel, & LeVay, Reference Hubel, Wiesel and LeVay1977), where all or none stimulation is needed for proper development. As a classic example, visual input programs the development of the ocular dominance columns such as those studied in kittens (Hubel et al., Reference Hubel, Wiesel and LeVay1977; Kuppermann & Kasamatsu, Reference Kuppermann and Kasamatsu1984). Sensitive periods occur after critical periods close and fine-tune the system to match the needs of the environment (e.g., neuronal imprinting). This process can be further broken down into an experience-dependent process, which is a more individualized process, or an experience-expectant process, where the brain is primed to certain aspects of environmental information (Greenough, Black, & Wallace, Reference Greenough, Black and Wallace1987). Inappropriate stimulation (too much, too little, or none at all) can shift the development of a phenotype that differs from typicality.

Exposure to adverse environments during sensitive periods of development will produce neuroanatomical and behavioral consequences that may be predictable if we understood all of the factors (Andersen & Teicher, Reference Andersen and Teicher2008; Cicchetti & Toth, Reference Cicchetti and Toth1995). Studies on the relationship among abuse, depression, and maturation support the hypothesis that clinical outcome depends on timing of the abusive episode. If an abusive episode occurs before 12 years of age, depression is a likely outcome; if the episode occurs after 12 years of age, posttraumatic stress disorder is more likely (Kaplow & Widom, Reference Kaplow and Widom2007; Schoedl et al., Reference Schoedl, Costa, Mari, Mello, Tyrka and Carpenter2010). A more recent study examined smaller sensitive periods to identify the strongest relationships between child abuse and depression (Dunn, McLaughlin, Slopen, Rosand, & Smoller, Reference Dunn, McLaughlin, Slopen, Rosand and Smoller2013). Data from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health demonstrate a significant relationship between physical abuse that occurred between 3 and 5 years of age and depression; suicidal ideation was higher in individuals sexually abused during preschool compared with adolescence. The data on risk for depression is consistent with a first wave of depressive risk that we observed in individuals with an early abuse history (Andersen et al., Reference Andersen, Tomada, Vincow, Valente, Polcari and Teicher2008; Andersen & Teicher, Reference Andersen and Teicher2008). Cortisol dysregulation is also stronger when physical or sexual abuse with depression or internalizing symptoms occurs earlier than when neglect or abuse plus depression occur together during adolescence (Cicchetti, Rogosch, Gunnar, & Toth, Reference Cicchetti, Rogosch, Gunnar and Toth2010).

Neuroanatomical studies show that abuse that occurs prior to puberty has more selective effects on the hippocampus, whereas abuse after puberty appears more selective for the prefrontal cortex in humans (Andersen & Teicher, Reference Andersen and Teicher2008). Studies on sensitive periods in typically developing children suggest that the optimal window for environmental impact on hippocampal development is before 8 years of age (Rao et al., Reference Rao, Betancourt, Giannetta, Brodsky, Korczykowski and Avants2010). In bonnet monkeys that were peer raised, significant reductions in the corpus callosum and hippocampus were observed during late adolescence (Jackowski et al., Reference Jackowski, Perera, Abdallah, Garrido, Tang and Martinez2011); these findings are consistent with those observed in humans (Andersen et al., Reference Andersen, Tomada, Vincow, Valente, Polcari and Teicher2008; Teicher et al., Reference Teicher, Dumont, Ito, Vaituzis, Giedd and Andersen2004). Animal research shows that different windows of early stress exposure produce unique behavioral (Freund, Thompson, Denormandie, Vacarro, & Andersen, Reference Freund, Thompson, Denormandie, Vaccarro and Andersen2013; Lehmann, Pryce, Bettschen, & Feldon, Reference Lehmann, Pryce, Bettschen and Feldon1999) and neuroanatomical effects (Leussis & Andersen, Reference Leussis and Andersen2008; Leussis, Freund, Brenhouse, Thompson, & Andersen, Reference Leussis, Freund, Brenhouse, Thompson and Andersen2012). These paradigms will be discussed in more detail below under the animal model section.

The nature of the abuse

The type of abuse (e.g., sexual, physical, or neglect) or nature of the early trauma can influence the brain region affected as well as the behavioral outcome (Tomoda et al., Reference Tomoda, Suzuki, Rabi, Sheu, Polcari and Teicher2009, Reference Tomoda, Sheu, Rabi, Suzuki, Navalta and Polcari2011; van Veen et al., Reference van Veen, Wardenaar, Carlier, Spinhoven, Penninx and Zitman2013). The nature of depressive symptoms differs (described in more detail below), although exposure to different types of stressors may provide some basis for their etiological underpinnings. Depression can present as either more anxious or anhedonic in children or adolescents (Mitchell, McCauley, Burke, & Moss, Reference Mitchell, McCauley, Burke and Moss1988). Adolescents with an anxious depression report more guilt, agitation, hypersomnia, and weight changes (gain or loss; Mitchell et al., Reference Mitchell, McCauley, Burke and Moss1988). Depression with conduct disorder also appears to be another subtype (Mitchell et al., Reference Mitchell, McCauley, Burke and Moss1988). A recent study examined specific outcomes in 2,615 individuals enrolled in the Netherlands Study for Depression and Anxiety (van Veen et al., Reference van Veen, Wardenaar, Carlier, Spinhoven, Penninx and Zitman2013). The authors found that emotional neglect had the greatest impact, affecting arousal and anhedonia dimensions. Physical and sexual abuse, as well as psychological abuse, was associated with a more anxious arousal; the former two were also associated with greater generalized arousal. Earlier studies show that physical abuse is associated with higher lifetime rates of major depression in women, but not men (MacMillan et al., Reference MacMillan, Fleming, Streiner, Lin, Boyle and Jamieson2001). Higher rates of psychopathology, including drug use, were higher in women following childhood sexual abuse than in men (MacMillan et al., Reference MacMillan, Fleming, Streiner, Lin, Boyle and Jamieson2001).

Nature of depressive symptoms

Depression is multifaceted, resulting from genetics, different environmental variables (including the timing of exposure to environmental factors), or both. A more extensive review of risk factors for depression can be found in Beardslee, Gladstone, and O'Connor (Reference Beardslee, Gladstone and O'Connor2012). Different treatment modalities support this notion, and depressive symptoms can include the following categories:

  1. 1. mood changes: sad, anxious, or “empty”; hopelessness, guilt, worthlessness, or helplessness; irritability; restlessness

  2. 2. anhedonia: a loss of interest in activities or things once pleasurable, including sex

  3. 3. changes in energy and feeling fatigued

  4. 4. working-memory problems and difficulty concentrating or making decisions

  5. 5. sleep–wake changes: insomnia, early-morning wakefulness, or excessive sleeping

  6. 6. changes in appetite

  7. 7. thoughts of suicide or suicide attempts

  8. 8. malaise: aches or pains, headaches, cramps, or digestive problems

Sex differences in depression

Sex differences in depression emerge during adolescence (Cyranowski, Frank, Young, & Shear, Reference Cyranowski, Frank, Young and Shear2000) and are maintained into adulthood. Depressive symptoms are often experienced differently between men and women. Men are more likely to report symptoms 2, 3, and 5 (above), whereas women usually endorse 1, 3, and 6 (Cochran, Reference Cochran2000; Pollack, Reference Pollack1998). The initiating events that can underlie depressive symptoms may also be sexually dimorphic. Depression in female teens is more strongly associated with maternal depressive behavior, whereas males are more strongly affected by changes in supportive early care (Duggal, Carlson, Sroufe, & Egeland, Reference Duggal, Carlson, Sroufe and Egeland2001).

Part of these changes may also be related to different sensitive periods in brain development (for a review, see Brenhouse & Andersen, Reference Brenhouse and Andersen2011). In general, males show greater overproduction and pruning in most brain regions compared to females (Giedd, Castellanos, Rajapakse, Vaituzis, & Rapoport, Reference Giedd, Castellanos, Rajapakse, Vaituzis and Rapoport1997). The process of overproduction and elimination may render males more vulnerable to environmental impacts (Andersen & Teicher, Reference Andersen and Teicher2009). Greater decreases in hippocampus and corpus callosum size are observed in males with a history of maltreatment than in females (DeBellis & Keshavan, Reference DeBellis and Keshavan2003; Teicher et al., Reference Teicher, Dumont, Ito, Vaituzis, Giedd and Andersen2004). Diurnal changes in cortisol levels also are higher in boys than in girls with a history of maltreatment (Doom, Cicchetti, Rogosch, & Dackis, Reference Doom, Cicchetti, Rogosch and Dackis2013).

Animal studies, however, have either not examined females in regard to stress and depression (Kaufman, Plotsky, Nemeroff, & Charney, Reference Kaufman, Plotsky, Nemeroff and Charney2000; Marais, van Rensburg, van Zyl, Stein, & Daniels, Reference Marais, van Rensburg, van Zyl, Stein and Daniels2008) or failed to find sex differences (discussed below). In genetic models bred to be more sensitive to depressive-like behavior, males, but not females, show greater immobility during adolescence (Mehta, Wang, & Redei, Reference Mehta, Wang and Redei2013). Females are more resistant to stressful events at the level of controllability that is related to escape behavior (Dalla, Edgecomb, Whetstone, & Shors, Reference Dalla, Edgecomb, Whetstone and Shors2007; Leussis & Andersen, Reference Leussis and Andersen2008; Leussis et al., Reference Leussis, Freund, Brenhouse, Thompson and Andersen2012). Consistent with the human literature, sex differences only became evident when we examined different aspects of depressive-like behavior in rats. Specifically, females showed slightly higher baseline levels of helplessness that are not what one would expect based on human studies. Two possibilities exist. One hypothesis is that human males underreport their depressive symptoms. However, findings from large, community-based samples suggest that such underreporting is not the case (Wolk & Weissman, Reference Wolk and Weissman1995) and that the reported 2:1 female to male sex difference in prevalence rate is found in the United States (Marcus et al., Reference Marcus, Young, Kerber, Kornstein, Farabaugh and Mitchell2005) and around the globe (Wolk & Weissman, Reference Wolk and Weissman1995). A second possibility is that the nature of what is measured in animals does not accurately reflect the human condition. In other words, the standard measures of depressive-like behavior in animals, the forced swim test (Detke, Rickels, & Lucki, Reference Detke, Rickels and Lucki1995), or the active avoidance paradigm (Olton, Johnson, & Howard, Reference Olton, Johnson and Howard1974), miss an important aspect of depression found in females.

While depressive-like behavior in females may be elusive in animal studies, estrogen and stress are known to interact and influence affective behaviors (Dalla, Pitychoutis, Kokras, & Papadopoulou-Daifoti, Reference Dalla, Pitychoutis, Kokras and Papadopoulou-Daifoti2011). In both animals and humans, low levels of estrogen are associated with greater depressive symptoms (Hajszan et al., Reference Hajszan, Szigeti-Buck, Sallam, Bober, Parducz and Maclusky2010; Young & Korszun, Reference Young and Korszun2010). Low or falling levels of estrogen typically occur during premenstrual dysphoria disorder and menopause, which are both associated with depressive symptoms. As estrogen modulates corticotropin releasing factor (CRF), a stress hormone, and serotonin (Bangasser & Valentino, Reference Bangasser and Valentino2012), stress-induced changes in estrogen are likely to modulate depressive behaviors. Developmentally, exposure to social stress during the juvenile period delays the onset of puberty in female nonhuman primates (Wilson et al., Reference Wilson, Bounar, Godfrey, Michopoulos, Higgins and Sanchez2013). As a result, estrogen levels do not rise during the normal developmental stage. Because estrogen increases dendritic spine density (Woolley, Gould, Frankfurt, & McEwen, Reference Woolley, Gould, Frankfurt and McEwen1990) that is instrumental for the overproduction of synapses during adolescence, changes in estrogen in response to stress can cause lifelong susceptibility to depressive behaviors that emerge during adolescence.

Animal Models of Early Adversity: Sensitive Periods and Socially Relevant Stress

A number of animal models have been developed to facilitate understanding changes in brain development. Generally, these paradigms are based on the social stage of the animal. Early developmental manipulations involve maternal care, whereas child to adolescent stage manipulations typically involve separation from peers. The latter manipulations can be escalated further by social defeat paradigms. By adulthood, exposure to stressful manipulations will temporarily perturb a system, but do not seem to permanently alter the course of its development. For example, both early life and adult stress exposure decrease dendritic spines that last into adulthood, but adult stress decreases the spines only temporarily and they regenerate within a 3-week time point. In contrast, growing up in a complex environment increases spine density (Volkmar & Greenough, Reference Volkmar and Greenough1972).

Maternal separation paradigms

Maternal care has a significant effect on subsequent development. Our initial interest in the neurobiological consequences of childhood maltreatment emerged from preclinical studies on the effects of early experience (e.g., Brunelli, Shindledecker, & Hofer, Reference Brunelli, Shindledecker and Hofer1989; Denenberg, Garbanati, Sherman, Yutzey, & Kaplan, Reference Denenberg, Garbanati, Sherman, Yutzey and Kaplan1978; Harlow & Harlow, Reference Harlow and Harlow1965; Levine, Johnson, & Gonzalez, Reference Levine, Johnson and Gonzalez1985). For rodent species, which are typically born relatively immature like humans, reliance on the dam for care is necessary for survival. Initially proposed as the “maternal mediation hypothesis” (Smotherman, Wiener, Mendoza, & Levine, Reference Smotherman, Wiener, Mendoza and Levine1977), the dam will adjust her care to match the environment in a way that maximizes survival. These experimental paradigms include manipulating environmental factors such as nest building material, foraging for food, and making resource availability unpredictable (primates; Rosenblum & Paully, Reference Rosenblum and Paully1984). Once these basic needs are met, the dam then focuses her time on the direct care of the offspring, including licking and grooming. If circumstances are favorable, she will often assume the more effective nursing posture of arched-back nursing (Anisman, Zaharia, Meaney, & Merali, Reference Anisman, Zaharia, Meaney and Merali1998; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Diorio, Tannenbaum, Caldji, Francis and Freedman1997). Pups that are raised by dams that demonstrate high licking and grooming behavior are more resilient to stress: cortisol and ACTH levels are reduced at baseline and in response to stress challenge later in life (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Diorio, Tannenbaum, Caldji, Francis and Freedman1997).

Paradigms that reduce arched-back nursing involve disruption of maternal care by separating the pups from the dam for various lengths of time. The original model by Levine (Reference Levine1967) used a 24-hr separation period, while the most common duration is 3–4 hr a day between Postnatal (P) Days 2–4 (P2–P14) or P2–P20 (Leussis et al., Reference Leussis, Freund, Brenhouse, Thompson and Andersen2012; Mourlon et al., Reference Mourlon, Baudin, Blanc, Lauber, Giros and Naudon2010; Plotsky & Meaney, Reference Plotsky and Meaney1993; Reus et al., Reference Reus, Stringari, Ribeiro, Cipriano, Panizzutti and Stertz2011). Variations exist within the maternal separation (MS) literature as well. We will use the designations of MS(24h) for separation for 24 hr, MS(3) for 3 hr a day between P2 and P14, or MS(4) for 4 hr a day between P2 and P20. Exceptions to the ages and/or times of separation for the different studies will be noted. In general, the MS(3) and MS(4) paradigms do not appear to differ significantly because they both include separation periods during the first week of life. The other factor that can differ among MS paradigms is whether the litter is separated as a unit from the dam or individual pups are separated from the dam and each other. While this distinction is not always clear, our unpublished observations suggest that the latter will have greater effects on the offspring.

The effectiveness of MS to produce changes in depression, fear, and anxiety depends on the timing of the paradigm as well as the species of animal. MS paradigms that include the first week but not the second week of life seem to produce differential effects. For example MS between P2 and P13 produces stronger fear responses to innate predator odors than in animals exposed to MS between P11 and P13 (Litvin et al., Reference Litvin, Tovote, Pentkowski, Zeyda, King and Vasconcellos2010). Similarly, exposure to saline injections at P9–P14 produces greater helplessness than injections at P2–P9 (Freund et al., Reference Freund, Thompson, Denormandie, Vaccarro and Andersen2013). The effects of MS can vary by strain of mice (Kundakovic, Lim, Gudsnuk, & Champagne, Reference Kundakovic, Lim, Gudsnuk and Champagne2013) or rat (Sterley, Howells, & Russell, Reference Sterley, Howells and Russell2011). For example, the spontaneously hypertensive rat is resistant to anxiogenic effects of MS, but demonstrates hyperactivity that has also been reported in abuse cases (Sterley et al., Reference Sterley, Howells and Russell2011). More examples will be given within different sections below.

Whether all of the effects on the offspring are “mediated” by dam as originally proposed is certainly debatable. A maternal “modulation” hypothesis has been put forth that states mom can play gatekeeper in her pup's exposure to novelty and stressors (Tang, Reeb-Sutherland, Romeo, & McEwen, Reference Tang, Reeb-Sutherland, Romeo and McEwen2013). This modulation is consistent with the findings of Macri, Mason, and Wurbel (Reference Macri, Mason and Wurbel2004), who describe dissociation among HPA responses, early life experiences, and fear responses.

Changes in rearing conditions

A number of paradigms can change rearing conditions, including changes in bedding (Coccurello et al., Reference Coccurello, Bielawski, Zelek-Molik, Vetulani, Kowalska and D'Amato2014), communal nursing paradigm in rats nesting (Macri, Laviola, Leussis, & Andersen, Reference Macri, Laviola, Leussis and Andersen2009) or in mice (Branchi & Alleva, Reference Branchi and Alleva2006), or reducing nest bedding (Raineki, Cortes, Belnoue, & Sullivan, Reference Raineki, Cortes, Belnoue and Sullivan2012). Collectively, these paradigms are designed to disrupt maternal behavior, or in the case of mice, elevate maternal behavior.

Social isolation paradigms

While removing a pup from the dam when very young is considered stressful, removal of pups from the dam when they are entering adolescence may not be as effective. Like humans, separation from peers is highly stressful. A number of social isolation paradigms are found in the literature, with the most common one being the isolation rearing paradigm. Pups are weaned from the dam and isolation housed typically from 28 to 55 days (Sahakian & Robbins, Reference Sahakian and Robbins1977; Warren & Ivinskis, Reference Warren and Ivinskis1973). During young adulthood, these subjects demonstrate a number of changes in behavior and HPA axis activity.

Other paradigms, including social defeat and chronic variable stress

The social defeat paradigm involves exposure of a subordinate rat to a dominant rat, whose dominance is established by previous bouts or age. A recent study subjected juvenile male and female rats to a dominant male adult for 10 min/day for 10 days (Weathington, Arnold, & Cooke, Reference Weathington, Arnold and Cooke2012).

The chronic variable stress paradigm includes twice-daily exposures to a number of different stressors, including crowding, social isolation, restraint, shaker stress, swimming, change in temperature, and air quality (Jankord et al., Reference Jankord, Solomon, Albertz, Flak, Zhang and Herman2011). While a number of these stressors can be used individually to produce chronic stress, the variability in their presentation typically produces different results (e.g., decreased HPA reactivity versus no change or elevation; Cruz, Marin, Leao, & Planeta, Reference Cruz, Marin, Leao and Planeta2012). This paradigm, like the MS paradigm, has variations in duration of exposure and the number of stressors used that are likely to make cross-study comparisons difficult.

Peer–peer rearing

Initially used in the classic study of Harlow and Harlow (Reference Harlow and Harlow1965), this model has been primarily used in studies of nonhuman primates (Bowden & McKinney, Reference Bowden and McKinney1972; Suomi, Harlow, & Domek, Reference Suomi, Harlow and Domek1970). These studies have been instrumental in illustrating the importance of age, rearing conditions, and rehousing during the stressor (Gilmer & McKinney, Reference Gilmer and McKinney2003).

Important Variables That Influence the Observed Results

Sex of the subject

A number of studies show that the effects of early exposure to adversity are sex dependent. Specific examples within each sensitive period of early adversity exposure are described within the next section.

Duration of the stressor

Clinical work shows that the duration of the stressor is often associated with worse outcomes (Beitchman et al., Reference Beitchman, Zucker, Hood, daCosta, Akman and Cassavia1992). However, disentangling the age of onset of the stressor from its duration is almost impossible, except in adult systems. Exposure to adversity early in development has a great impact not only on the foundational maturation of a number of systems, including basic behavioral processes, but also on the underlying neural structures. Alterations in behavior affecting basic self-regulatory processes that occur during the first few years of life are likely to lead to other issues later in life (Cicchetti & Rogosch, Reference Cicchetti and Rogosch2002). Similarly, exposure to early adversity is less evident during the building-up phase of hippocampal development, but without basic synaptic structure present before pruning, some minimal amount of foundation is lost and becomes evident during adolescence when excess synapses are pruned away (Andersen & Teicher, Reference Andersen and Teicher2004).

Timing of assessment postadversity

Does the clock start at the age of initiation or the age of termination? Increasing cross-talk among disciplines (e.g., developmental psychologists, clinical outreach workers, and prevention scientists) will facilitate early identification of psychopathology, if it is going to manifest, earlier. Depression often appears as children enter adolescence. In our studies (Andersen, Tordera, Lasheras, Del Rio, & Ramirez, 2008; Teicher et al., Reference Teicher, Samson, Polcari and Andersen2009) and others (Widom et al., Reference Widom, DuMont and Czaja2007), depression and substance-use disorder are among the most likely outcomes (Kaplow & Widom, Reference Kaplow and Widom2007). The delay in the diagnosis for depression in our sample was ~9.2 years, unless the abuse occurred during adolescence. Childhood maltreatment accelerates the onset of depression relative to the typical age of onset in nonmaltreated individuals. Earlier studies that examined the expression of depressive symptoms within 8 weeks of an abusive episode demonstrated that adolescent maltreatment is associated with the appearance of greater depressive symptoms (reviewed by Feiring, Taska, & Lewis, Reference Feiring, Taska and Lewis1999). However, this conclusion is likely incorrect given the extensive work in animals and humans reviewed in this paper that suggests interactive effects between maltreatment and maturation. In other words, children with a maltreatment history are strongly at risk for the development of depression too.

These data suggest that the brain must reach some maturational stage that permits the expression of the underlying pathology. A number of possibilities may explain this delay. First, the physical change in synapses must reach a certain level of loss in order for the deficit to be observed. Parkinson disease, for example, requires >90% loss of dopamine neurons before the patient experiences symptoms. Second, maturational processes need to occur to unmask the deficit. These processes may involve either the failure to overproduce synapses or the pruning of excess synapses, such as what we have observed in MS(4) subjects as they enter adulthood (e.g., (Andersen & Teicher, Reference Andersen and Teicher2004); or age-related changes in connectivity between brain regions. A number of anxiety-driven responses that are associated with depression involve reciprocal connections between the amygdala and the prefrontal cortex (Cressman et al., Reference Cressman, Balaban, Steinfeld, Shemyakin, Graham and Parisot2010; Cunningham, Bhattacharyya, & Benes, Reference Cunningham, Bhattacharyya and Benes2002). In rats, amygdalar–cortical projections increase in age up until P60 (late adolescence; Cunningham et al., Reference Cunningham, Bhattacharyya and Benes2002). While they appear to stabilize with age, projections from the prefrontal cortex to the basolateral amygdala prune into adulthood (Cressman et al., Reference Cressman, Balaban, Steinfeld, Shemyakin, Graham and Parisot2010). Connections between the prefrontal cortex and the accumbens may have similar developmental effects on the appearance of anhedonia (Brenhouse, Sonntag, & Andersen, Reference Brenhouse, Sonntag and Andersen2008).

Increases in EEG coherence are evident in limbic regions of individuals with a history of maltreatment, including both child and adult trauma in humans (Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, Herman, Barrett, Noble, Wojteczko and Chisholm2009). Consistent with a number of the depressive features listed below, resting-state functional connectivity in areas related to episodic memory and retrieval is impaired (van der Werff et al., Reference van der Werff, Pannekoek, Veer, van Tol, Aleman and Veltman2013b). In contrast, areas related to declarative memory and stronger emotional processing show stronger resting-state connectivity between the dorsal anterior cingulate and regions containing the lingual and orbital fusiform gyri in individuals who experienced maltreatment but were resilient (van der Werff et al., Reference van der Werff, Pannekoek, Veer, van Tol, Aleman and Veltman2013a).

Only recently has clinical research revealed that cortisol levels or its regulation by ACTH in response to CRF challenge transition with either further maturation or time since the last episode of abuse. Children exposed to early adversity demonstrate elevated levels of cortisol initially during or immediately following the abusive episode (Bevans, Cerbone, & Overstreet, Reference Bevans, Cerbone and Overstreet2008; Carrion & Wong, Reference Carrion and Wong2012; DeBellis et al., Reference DeBellis, Narasimhan, Thatcher, Keshavan, Soloff and Clark2005). Later, these elevations give way to blunted responses by adulthood or even shortly after the abuse ends (reviewed in Struber, Struber, & Roth, Reference Struber, Struber and Roth2013). This transition makes sense, given that elevated cortisol will facilitate fight or flight. Over time, the allostatic load becomes too great for the system to bear and, eventually, cortisol regulation is blunted.

Alternatively, some behaviors may require full maturation to occur before manifesting. Animals that underwent MS for 3 hr during postnatal development showed significantly more time spent in the open arm of an elevated plus maze when assessed at P37, but less time when assessed at P54 (Feng et al., Reference Feng, Sheng, Li, Wang, Ren and Jin2013).

Part of the delay in the appearance of behavioral changes is the result of delayed maturation. For example, a loss in gray matter volume in the hippocampus has been well documented in adults with a history of childhood maltreatment (Bremner et al., Reference Bremner, Randall, Vermetten, Staib, Bronen and Mazure1997; DeBellis et al., Reference DeBellis, Keshavan, Clark, Casey, Gledd and Boring1999) and in animals that were exposed to early life stress (Andersen & Teicher, Reference Andersen and Teicher2008; Huot, Plotsky, Lenox, & McNamara, Reference Huot, Plotsky, Lenox and McNamara2002). When children were examined for volume changes in the hippocampus, none were observed (DeBellis et al., Reference DeBellis, Keshavan, Clark, Casey, Gledd and Boring1999). Longitudinal studies now show that the loss of hippocampal volume is progressive (Tupler & DeBellis, Reference Tupler and DeBellis2006). These delayed effects are not unique to humans. Rhesus monkeys that were peer raised did not show changes in the hippocampus or corpus callosum when assessed at 24–30 months (~6–8 years human), although enlargements in the cerebellar vermis and cortical regions were found (Spinelli et al., Reference Spinelli, Chefer, Suomi, Higley, Barr and Stein2009). Hippocampal and corpus callosum decreases were documented in peer-reared bonnet monkeys that were assessed at an older age (Jackowski et al., Reference Jackowski, Perera, Abdallah, Garrido, Tang and Martinez2011). Similarly, no difference in hippocampal synaptophysin (a measure of synapses) was evident until after the onset of puberty in rats exposed to MS(4) (Andersen & Teicher, Reference Andersen and Teicher2004). These data imply that maltreatment interacts with maturation for the full effect to manifest.

Clinical and Clinically Relevant Findings

Depression cross-species

Mood changes: Sad, anxious, or “empty”; hopelessness, guilt, worthlessness, or helplessness; irritability; restlessness

Depression often emerges during young adolescence following exposure to early life adversity (Teicher et al., Reference Teicher, Samson, Polcari and Andersen2009; Widom et al., Reference Widom, DuMont and Czaja2007), with few depressive symptoms observed in childhood. Feelings of worthlessness and guilt have been reported in females with a maltreatment history (Schuck & Widom, Reference Schuck and Widom2001). Receiving welfare as a child and living with alcoholic parents increase the strength of the relationship between abuse and worthlessness. Victims of bullying report greater internalizing behaviors (miserable, fearful, worried, solitary, apathetic, and not liked), whereas the bullies rate higher externalizing scores (lies, irritability, steals, fights, not liked, disobedient, and destructive) based on the Rutter scale (Kumpulainen et al., Reference Kumpulainen, Rasanen, Henttonen, Almqvist, Kresanov and Linna1998).

Anxiety and fear

Nonhuman primates that were reared by peers and not their mother demonstrated increased fear-potentiated startle as juveniles (Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, Herman, Barrett, Noble, Wojteczko and Chisholm2009). The effects of maternal separation early in life, for as little as one or two 6-day separations, persist in primates' approach to novelty in adulthood (Hinde, Spencer-Booth, & Bruce, Reference Hinde, Spencer-Booth and Bruce1966).

Changes in fear and anxiety are evident in the animal models, but inconsistently (discussed in Stevenson, Meredith, Spicer, Mason, & Marsden, Reference Stevenson, Meredith, Spicer, Mason and Marsden2009). One of the plausible explanations for these inconsistencies points to the amount of learning the assessment task requires. MS subjects in general show some degree of impaired learning of fear-related tasks (Aisa et al., Reference Aisa, Tordera, Lasheras, Del Rio and Ramirez2008). Regardless, a number of studies show decreased time in the open arm of the elevated plus maze, which is an index of anxiety-like behavior. MS(3) decreases open arm time in adulthood (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Kim, Yim, Kim, Kim and Kim2001) in both males and females, although the effects are greater in males (Wigger & Neumann, Reference Wigger and Neumann1999). Communal nursing in both rats (Macri et al., Reference Macri, Laviola, Leussis and Andersen2009) and mice (Branchi & Alleva, Reference Branchi and Alleva2006) also increase anxiety-like behavior. Our study in rats demonstrated reduced maternal care the first few days of life, which may be sufficient to produce lasting changes in affect (Macri et al., Reference Macri, Laviola, Leussis and Andersen2009). These findings are consistent with other paradigms that manipulate maternal cues (Moriceau, Wilson, Levine, & Sullivan, Reference Moriceau, Wilson, Levine and Sullivan2006).

Isolation rearing increased anxiety-like behavior in the elevated plus maze that was not reversed upon resocialization (Wright, Upton, & Marsden, Reference Wright, Upton and Marsden1991). Similarly, isolation rearing increased fear reactivity and social anxiety in early adulthood in male rats (Lukkes, Vuong, Scholl, Oliver, & Forster, Reference Lukkes, Vuong, Scholl, Oliver and Forster2009; Lukkes, Watt, Lowry, & Forster, Reference Lukkes, Watt, Lowry and Forster2009). Isolation rearing enhances anxiety-like behavior in females (Da Silva, Ferreira, Carobrez Ade, & Morato, Reference Da Silva, Ferreira, Carobrez Ade and Morato1996). Anxiety-like behavior also depended on species and testing conditions; isolation rearing increased anxiety more in Fawn Hooded rats than in Wistar rats under low light conditions, but Wistars were less anxious when the lights were brighter (Hall, Huang, Fong, Pert, & Linnoila, Reference Hall, Huang, Fong, Pert and Linnoila1998). In a different test of anxiety, defensive marble burying was reduced in both males and females that were isolated during adolescence (Arakawa, Reference Arakawa2007).

Chronic variable stress during adolescence did not affect anxiety-like behavior (plus maze) in either males or females during adolescence (Taylor, Taylor, & Koenig, Reference Taylor, Taylor and Koenig2013). However, this same paradigm reduced freezing in females, but had no effect in males (Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Taylor and Koenig2013).

Depressive-like behavior

Nonhuman primates that were isolated during development show depressive-like behaviors (Laudenslager, Held, Boccia, Reite, & Cohen, Reference Laudenslager, Held, Boccia, Reite and Cohen1990; Suomi, Delizio, & Harlow, Reference Suomi, Delizio and Harlow1976). Depressive-like behaviors include postural changes and locomotor retardation (others are specifically discussed below). These behaviors are reversible if the monkeys are rehoused with their peers (Suomi et al., Reference Suomi, Delizio and Harlow1976).

The forced swim test (FST) has been used to test antidepressant efficacy, and by proxy, depressive-like behavior (Detke et al., Reference Detke, Rickels and Lucki1995). Increased depressive-like symptoms occur in the MS model (Matthews, Wilkinson, & Robbins, Reference Matthews, Wilkinson and Robbins1996). MS(3) with the whole litter separated as a unit increased the time spent immobile in adulthood in Wistar (Aisa et al., Reference Aisa, Tordera, Lasheras, Del Rio and Ramirez2008), Fischer (Ruedi-Bettschen et al., Reference Ruedi-Bettschen, Zhang, Russig, Ferger, Weston and Pedersen2006), and Sprague-Dawley rats (Marais, van Rensburg, van Zyl, Stein, & Daniels, Reference Marais, van Rensburg, van Zyl, Stein and Daniels2008); Flinders Sensitive Line (FSL) of rats also show greater depressive-like behavior that depends on the quality of maternal care (Friedman, Berman, & Overstreet, Reference Friedman, Berman and Overstreet2006). If FSL rats were cross-fostered to the more nurturing Flinders Resistant strain, the depressive effects were less than if raised by FSL dams. Similarly, increased maternal care following brief handling made males more resistant to immobility later in life (Papaioannou, Gerozissis, Prokopiou, Bolaris, & Stylianopoulou, Reference Papaioannou, Gerozissis, Prokopiou, Bolaris and Stylianopoulou2002).

The ability of the medial prefrontal cortex to exert behavioral control over a stressor plays a critical role in protecting the brain from depression (Maier & Watkins, Reference Maier and Watkins2005; Robbins, Reference Robbins2005). Loss of such control results in hopelessness, which is strongly associated with depression and posttraumatic stress disorder (Maier & Watkins, Reference Maier and Watkins2005). MS(4) increases depressive-like symptoms in the triadic model of learned helplessness (Freund et al., Reference Freund, Thompson, Denormandie, Vaccarro and Andersen2013; Leussis et al., Reference Leussis, Freund, Brenhouse, Thompson and Andersen2012). Specifically, males show a greater loss of controllability over their perceived ability to escape their situation (e.g., footshock); in contrast, females show greater deficits in their motivation to escape footshock upon first exposure (Leussis et al., Reference Leussis, Freund, Brenhouse, Thompson and Andersen2012). Sensitive periods that are sex sensitive exist before weaning and are expressed during adolescence. We found that males showed more helplessness in response to saline injections at P9–P14, than at P2–P9; conversely, females showed increased resiliency when given a daily saline injection between P2 and P9, but only a modest effect at P9–P14 (Freund et al., Reference Freund, Thompson, Denormandie, Vaccarro and Andersen2013). The effects of MS on the FST were minimal in Long–Evans rats (Mourlon et al., Reference Mourlon, Baudin, Blanc, Lauber, Giros and Naudon2010).

When rats were housed in isolation between 30 and 35 days of age and then tested 2 days later in adolescence, the male rats demonstrated learned helplessness and the females spent more time in the closed arm of the elevated plus maze (Leussis & Andersen, Reference Leussis and Andersen2008). Isolation rearing for longer periods (weaning to late adolescence) in rats increased immobility in the FST, although these subjects also received saline injections during preweanling stages (Kuramochi & Nakamura, Reference Kuramochi and Nakamura2009). The same results were observed in noninjected social isolates (Brenes, Padilla, & Fornaguera, Reference Brenes, Padilla and Fornaguera2009), although no enduring effect on immobility has been reported (Hong et al., Reference Hong, Flashner, Chiu, ver Hoeve, Luz and Bhatnagar2012). These effects were lower in animals raised in an enriched environment. In contrast, no effect on swimming behavior was observed in Wistar or Fawn-Hooded rats that were isolated and tested in adulthood (Hall et al., Reference Hall, Huang, Fong, Pert and Linnoila1998). Isolation-reared rats also show reduced exploration of novelty (File, Reference File1978).

The use of social defeat paradigms to examine depressive-like behavior is a relatively young field. Female adolescent rats exposed to 7 days of consecutive social defeat demonstrated transient depressive behaviors in the FST (Ver Hoeve, Kelly, Luz, Ghanshani, & Bhatnagar, Reference Ver Hoeve, Kelly, Luz, Ghanshani and Bhatnagar2013). In contrast, exposure to a dominant male for 10 min/day for 10 days during the juvenile period produced lasting depressive-like effects in females, but not males, in adulthood (Weathington et al., Reference Weathington, Arnold and Cooke2012). Similar to the transient effects on FST behavior if social defeat occurred during adolescence, exposure to chronic variable stress during early or late adolescence failed to affect immobility in the FST; this stressor increased immobility in adult-exposed subjects only (Jankord et al., Reference Jankord, Solomon, Albertz, Flak, Zhang and Herman2011).

Anhedonia: A loss of interest in activities or things once pleasurable, including sex

Only a few studies have examined anhedonia in adolescents or adults with a history of childhood maltreatment. One such study reported elevated anhedonia based on the Mood and Anxiety Symptoms Questionnaire in individuals when the abuse occurred before 14 years of age (Weathington et al., Reference Weathington, Arnold and Cooke2012).

Anhedonia in animals is measured with a variety of paradigms, including decreased consumption of sucrose solutions, decreased sexual activity, and a rightward shift in intracranial self-stimulation currents (ICSS) where more current is needed to activate reward systems (Kornetsky & Esposito, Reference Kornetsky and Esposito1979). MS(3) decreased responding to novel environments (Matthews, Wilkinsin, et al., Reference Matthews, Hall, Wilkinson and Robbins1996). MS(3) rats also show reduced sucrose drinking compared with controls, and this reduction did not differ across males and females in both a positive and negative contrast challenge, where subjects were exposed to a low/high percentage of sucrose followed by a higher/lower one, respectively (Matthews, Hall, Wilkinson, & Robbins, Reference Matthews, Hall, Wilkinson and Robbins1996). Similarly, MS(3) Wistar rats also worked less for a sucrose reward in adulthood (Leventopoulos, Russig, Feldon, Pryce, & Opacka-Juffry, Reference Leventopoulos, Russig, Feldon, Pryce and Opacka-Juffry2009). In a slight modification of the MS(3) paradigm, mice separated from the dam and placed on clean bedding between P1 and P14 reduced saccharin drinking in adulthood (Coccurello et al., Reference Coccurello, Bielawski, Zelek-Molik, Vetulani, Kowalska and D'Amato2014).

Sucrose drinking was increased in a positive contrast design in rats that were raised in social isolation from weaning until young adulthood (Hall, Humby, Wilkinson, & Robbins, Reference Hall, Humby, Wilkinson and Robbins1997). In a separate study, these effects were not observed in males, but in female adult rats that were isolated (Hong et al., Reference Hong, Flashner, Chiu, ver Hoeve, Luz and Bhatnagar2012). These effects may also depend on the strain of rat used. The Wistar Kyoto rat has been used in previous studies of depression (Overstreet, Reference Overstreet2012). When this strain is exposed to social isolation between 27 and 49 days, females have reduced sucrose consumption compared to controls both immediately after the stressor in late adolescence or in adulthood; males did not show any change (Bourke & Neigh, Reference Bourke and Neigh2011). Taken together, these data suggest that social stress manipulations on sucrose drinking depend on the age of manipulation as well as the strain of rat.

Measures of ICSS for anhedonia suggest that the MS effect is modest. Rats that underwent MS(3) between P1 and P14 failed to show an anhedonic-like shift in ICSS currents under baseline conditions (Der-Avakian & Markou, Reference Der-Avakian and Markou2010; Matthews & Robbins, Reference Matthews and Robbins2003), but required greater current following 7 days of repeated social defeat (Der-Avakian & Markou, Reference Der-Avakian and Markou2010). These data suggest that MS produces a subthreshold effect that requires additional stress for an underlying effect to emerge. In another study, MS(3) rats tested in adulthood demonstrated reduced ICSS response rates in females only with no change between MS(3) males and controls (Michaels, Easterling, & Holtzman, Reference Michaels, Easterling and Holtzman2007). A subset of isolation-reared rats show equivocal changes to ICSS thresholds that are modulated by D2 receptors (Sundstrom, Hall, Stellar, & Waugh, Reference Sundstrom, Hall, Stellar and Waugh2002).

In general, appetitive behavior decreases following early life MS, whereas social isolations increase appetitive behavior (Harmer & Phillips, Reference Harmer and Phillips1998). However, this relationship may also depend on when the behavior is measured. Rhesus monkeys that were peer raised, which produces stressful-like effects, demonstrated an increase in sweet drinking as juveniles (Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, Herman, Barrett, Noble, Wojteczko and Chisholm2009); maternally deprived rhesus monkeys had reduced sweet drinking in adulthood (Paul, English, & Halaris, Reference Paul, English and Halaris2000).

Changes in energy and feeling fatigued

A history of childhood sexual abuse is predictive of levels of fatigue (Taylor & Jason, Reference Taylor and Jason2002). Abuse that occurred during childhood has a stronger association with levels of fatigue than when abuse occurred during adolescence. Childhood maltreatment, independent of type, increases risk for chronic fatigue syndrome three- to eightfold (Heim et al., Reference Heim, Wagner, Maloney, Papanicolaou, Solomon and Jones2006). These relationships are all further associated with elevated levels of depression and anxiety, and stronger if criteria for posttraumatic stress syndrome are met (Heim et al., Reference Heim, Nater, Maloney, Boneva, Jones and Reeves2009).

Working-memory problems and difficulty concentrating or making decisions

The ability to maintain negative instead of positive affective information when processing emotional material is associated with an increased risk of depression. In multiple studies of individuals with depression and a history of maltreatment, this negativity bias is a common finding (Goodman, Quas, & Ogle, Reference Goodman, Quas and Ogle2010). Young women (mean age = 20 years) with a history of childhood maltreatment have difficulty maintaining positively valenced information relative to nonmaltreated controls (Cromheeke, Herpoel, & Mueller, 2013). In addition, longer duration of abuse is positively associated with memory impairment (Navalta, Polcairi, Webster, Boghossian, & Teicher, Reference Navalta, Polcari, Webster, Boghossian and Teicher2006). These results held in an adult population-based study, where greater memory impairment was associated with greater adverse childhood experiences (ACEs; Brown et al., Reference Brown, Anda, Edwards, Felitti, Dube and Giles2007).

Working-memory impairment has been observed in both MS(3) and MS(4) subjects tested prepubertally or during adolescence, respectively (Brenhouse & Andersen, Reference Brenhouse and Andersen2011; Frankola et al., Reference Frankola, Flora, Torres, Grissom, Overstreet and Dohanich2010). MS rats showed more errors and greater latency on the win-shift paradigm during adolescence, but the effect was no longer apparent in adulthood (Brenhouse & Andersen, Reference Brenhouse and Andersen2011). The effects on memory were also specific for males, because MS(3) females did not show impairment when tested prepubertally (Frankola et al., Reference Frankola, Flora, Torres, Grissom, Overstreet and Dohanich2010). Isolation rearing impairs working and spatial memory in adult gerbils (Winterfeld, Teuchert-Noodt, & Dawirs, Reference Winterfeld, Teuchert-Noodt and Dawirs1998) and in rats (Quan, Tian, Xu, Zhang, & Wang, Reference Quan, Tian, Xu, Zhang and Yang2010).

Relative to hooded rats raised in an enriched environment, rats raised in isolation after weaning have more errors in a radial arm maze (Juraska, Henderson, & Muller, Reference Juraska, Henderson and Muller1984). No sex differences were evident. Similar findings were found in Wistar rats (Gorisch & Schwarting, Reference Gorisch and Schwarting2006).

Sleep–wake changes: Insomnia, early-morning wakefulness, or excessive sleeping

Sleep difficulties are a common occurrence in children and adolescents with depression, such that insomnia is more prevalent than hypersomnia (Morielli, Ladan, Ducharme, & Brouillette, Reference Morielli, Ladan, Ducharme and Brouillette1996). Posttraumatic stress disorder includes reexperiencing and nightmares as a factor in diagnosis. In adults with a history of child maltreatment, three fourths report significant sleep disturbances (Krakow et al., Reference Krakow, Melendrez, Johnston, Warner, Clark and Pacheco2002). However, the relationship between the type of abuse and sleep disturbance may be surprising. Consistent with the notion of a maturational delay in symptoms following abuse, sleep disturbances have been reported to emerge years after the abuse (Trickett, Noll, Reiffman, & Putnam, Reference Trickett, Noll, Reiffman and Putnam2001). In cases where the abuse was deemed “less serious,” the sleep disturbances were greater (Noll, Trickett, Susman, & Putnam, Reference Noll, Trickett, Susman and Putnam2006). Such children may have been older, exposed to little violence, a shorter duration of abuse, or not closely related to the perpetrator. Other reports on sleep difficulties suggest that the type of abuse or perpetrator does not significantly matter (Rimsza, Berg, & Locke, Reference Rimsza, Berg and Locke1988). A likely explanation is that children with “less serious abuse” may not have received sufficient treatment, possibly because they may have seemed asymptomatic (known as sleeper effects; Briere, Reference Briere1992).

Early research has shown that basic sleep–wake behavior is modulated by maternal behavior and milk delivery (Hofer, Reference Hofer1975). MS(3) rats demonstrate greater REM sleep (~35% more than controls) and more time sleeping than controls (Kinkead, Montandon, Bairam, Lajeunesse, & Horner, Reference Kinkead, Montandon, Bairam, Lajeunesse and Horner2009). These findings have been replicated (Mrdalj et al., Reference Mrdalj, Pallesen, Milde, Jellestad, Murison and Ursin2013). No effect on sleep architecture was observed in Wistar rats that were exposed to prenatal stress (Dugovic, Maccari, Weibel, Turek, & Van Reeth, Reference Dugovic, Maccari, Weibel, Turek and Van Reeth1999). Social isolation later in life (P50–P60) had little effect on sleep architecture in rats, although living within an enriched environment improves sleep quality (Kiyono, Seo, & Shibagaki, Reference Kiyono, Seo and Shibagaki1981).

Changes in appetite

Clinical and preclinical studies show a relationship between the postnatal environment and the risk of developing obesity (Felitti, Reference Felitti1991; Vamosi, Heitmann, & Kyvik, Reference Vamosi, Heitmann and Kyvik2010). The risk is higher in girls. Exposure to chronic stress may lead to “comfort” eating, where children and adolescents are likely to overeat. Elevations in cortisol can elevate insulin secretion, ultimately leading to its dysregulation; together, these factors further enhance appetite and/or weight gain (Pervanidou & Chrousos, Reference Pervanidou and Chrousos2011). Binge eating has been associated with greater weight gain in victims of childhood sexual abuse (Gustafson & Sarwer, Reference Gustafson and Sarwer2004).

Changes in growth and eating behavior are programmed early in life (Kuhn, Pauk, & Schanberg, Reference Kuhn, Pauk and Schanberg1990; Plotsky & Meaney, Reference Plotsky and Meaney1993). Maternal behavior affects growth hormone, corticosterone, and other hormones involved in development in rats (Kuhn, Butler, & Schanberg, Reference Kuhn, Butler and Schanberg1978) and in nonhuman primates (Kaufman et al., Reference Kaufman, Banerji, Shorman, Smith, Coplan and Rosenblum2007). For example, leptin, a hormone involved in regulating energy intake and use (including thermogenesis), peaks at ~P10 in rats before declining (Blumberg, Deaver, & Kirby, Reference Blumberg, Deaver and Kirby1999; Delahaye et al., Reference Delahaye, Breton, Risold, Enache, Dutriez-Casteloot and Laborie2008). Leptin can modify the effects of MS by reducing responsivity to external ACTH (Salzmann et al., Reference Salzmann, Otis, Long, Roberge, Gallo-Payet and Walker2004). Given that MS pups show reduced rates of growth before puberty (Freund et al., Reference Freund, Thompson, Denormandie, Vaccarro and Andersen2013), early life changes in leptin make MS subjects less vulnerable to gaining weight in adulthood when fed a high-fat diet (Paternain et al., Reference Paternain, Martisova, Milagro, Ramirez, Martinez and Campion2012). However, male MS subjects fed a regular chow diet showed increased weight gain in adulthood and females did not (Matthews, Wilkinson, et al., Reference Matthews, Hall, Wilkinson and Robbins1996); this weight gain following a chow diet or one deficient in omega 3s, in which insulin resistance biomarkers are decreased (Bernardi et al., Reference Bernardi, Ferreira, Senter, Krolow, de Aguiar and Portella2013; Delaunay et al., Reference Delaunay, Khan, Cintra, Davani, Ling and Andersson1997; Lambillotte, Gilon, & Henquin, Reference Lambillotte, Gilon and Henquin1997; Solas et al., Reference Solas, Aisa, Mugueta, Del Rio, Tordera and Ramirez2010).

Rats exposed to MS(3), and then subjected to social isolation, gained more weight than either manipulation alone (Ryu, Yoo, Kang, Lee, & Jahng, Reference Ryu, Yoo, Kang, Lee and Jahng2009). In contrast, rats exposed to a chronic variable stress paradigm during early or late adolescence or adulthood lost weight in general, as well as fat weight specifically (Jankord et al., Reference Jankord, Solomon, Albertz, Flak, Zhang and Herman2011; Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Taylor and Koenig2013); others have reported no weight loss (Cruz et al., Reference Cruz, Marin, Leao and Planeta2012).

Thoughts of suicide, suicide attempts

Early ACEs significantly increase the risk of attempted suicide by two to five times, raising the rates from the general population prevalence of 1.1% to as high as 35% if seven or more ACEs are present (Dube et al., Reference Dube, Anda, Felitti, Chapman, Williamson and Giles2001). A recent longitudinal study in New Zealand that followed 900 individuals less than 30 years of age demonstrated a significant relationship between childhood sexual abuse and both suicidal ideation and attempts (Fergusson, McLeod, & Horwood, Reference Fergusson, McLeod and Horwood2013). Of note, elevated suicidality is also evident in individuals with a history of physical abuse (Fuller-Thomson, Baker, & Brennenstuhl, Reference Fuller-Thomson, Baker and Brennenstuhl2012). Other studies have found similar results in teenagers (Klomek et al., Reference Klomek, Kleinman, Altschuler, Marrocco, Amakawa and Gould2013). The study of suicide, attempts, and ideation in animals at this stage is impossible.

Malaise: Aches or pains, headaches, cramps, or digestive problems

Digestion problems, including nausea, stomachaches, and vomiting, have been associated with early abuse in boys and girls (Kugler, Bloom, Kaercher, Truax, & Storch, Reference Kugler, Bloom, Kaercher, Truax and Storch2012). Digestive issues have been documented in animals that experienced MS and are reviewed elsewhere (Barreau, Ferrier, Fioramonti, & Bueno, Reference Barreau, Ferrier, Fioramonti and Bueno2007).

Treatment Implications

Exposure to adversity early in life is associated with increased susceptibility to other disorders in adulthood besides mental illness, including cancer, diabetes mellitus, and obesity (Burdge, Lillycrop, & Jackson, Reference Burdge, Lillycrop and Jackson2009; Felitti et al., Reference Felitti, Anda, Nordenberg, Williamson, Spitz and Edwards1998). A greater understanding about how important variables an many different levels of analysis contribute to the negative consequences of adversity exposure can be used to design interventions (Cicchetti & Gunnar, Reference Cicchetti and Gunnar2008). Such variables include the duration of the stressor, the immediacy or lack thereof of consequences, and the transitions that occur during the course of the resultant shifts in brain development. Discussion of these variables from a translational perspective may shed light on the gaps in our knowledge. As we (Andersen, Reference Andersen2005; Stanis & Andersen, in press) and others (Fishbein, Reference Fishbein2000; Studts & van Zyl, Reference Studts and van Zyl2013) have discussed elsewhere, interventions need to be developmentally appropriate for the subject, specific to the modality affected, and well timed to maximize their effects (Wachs, Georgieff, Cusick, & McEwen, Reference Wachs, Georgieff, Cusick and McEwen2013).

Maternal behavior has a significant influence on the outcome of fear, anxiety, and depressive behaviors. A recent clinical study found that reducing the child's exposure to a depressed mother (presumably a condition that reduces her caregiving) by placing the child in daycare was associated with a significant reduction in internalizing behaviors (Herba et al., Reference Herba, Tremblay, Boivin, Liu, Mongeau and Seguin2013). Programs that aim to increase support to a mother who is experiencing domestic abuse, which is likely to reduce her ability to care for her child and expose the child to a traumatizing event, may further provide an effective intervention to break the chain of abuse (Taft et al., Reference Taft, Small, Hegarty, Watson, Gold and Lumley2011). The beneficial effects of brief separations during development have been documented where brief handling for 15 min daily in rats reduces stress responsiveness (Plotsky et al., Reference Plotsky, Thrivikraman, Nemeroff, Caldji, Sharma and Meaney2005) and improves spatial working memory and novel object recognition in female rats (Plescia et al., Reference Plescia, Marino, Navarra, Gambino, Brancato and Sardo2013). Even improved environments during pregnancy may have positive benefits. Environmental enrichment during pregnancy and lactation had long-lasting effects on reactivity to acute and chronic stress in offspring that were not highly dependent on early postpartum maternal behavior (Welberg, Thrivikraman, & Plotsky, Reference Welberg, Thrivikraman and Plotsky2006). A number of intervention approaches for older children with a history of maltreatment are being developed (reviewed by Toth & Cicchetti, Reference Toth and Cicchetti2013).

Interventions would benefit greatly from improved reporting of abusive experiences. A single episode of abuse is associated with more favorable outcomes than repeated exposures (Copeland, Keeler, Angold, & Costello, Reference Copeland, Keeler, Angold and Costello2007). Unfortunately, females are more likely to experience higher rates of revictimization of abuse, which may explain higher rates of depression in females (Koenen & Widom, Reference Koenen and Widom2009). Secondary to increased revictimization, a window of opportunity exists for intervention even though the individual is not symptomatic (Teicher et al., Reference Teicher, Samson, Polcari and Andersen2009). As depression is accelerated into early adolescence following maltreatment (Gladstone et al., Reference Gladstone, Parker, Mitchell, Malhi, Wilhelm and Austin2004), symptoms do not emerge until this period. In our clinical sample, we found that an average of 9.2 years lapsed between the initial abusive event and depression onset (Teicher et al., Reference Teicher, Samson, Polcari and Andersen2009).

This review highlighted the historical and current understanding of the effects of social stress manipulations on behaviors related to depression. While much research has been conducted in this area, a greater understanding of the specific aspects of depression is still warranted. A number of factors need to be considered when identifying the individual's degree of risk to develop depression (e.g., genetics, gender, age of abuse, and duration of abuse), as well as what symptoms to predict. Early life stress seems to more strongly influence anxiety, mood, and anhedonia, whereas later life stress appears to have greater effects on externalizing behaviors, which were not discussed in this review. Early interventions (Wachs et al., Reference Wachs, Georgieff, Cusick and McEwen2013) that consider the implications of any or all of these factors are likely to impact depression reduction.

References

Aisa, B., Tordera, R., Lasheras, B., Del Rio, J. & Ramirez, M. J. (2008). Effects of maternal separation on hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal responses, cognition and vulnerability to stress in adult female rats. Neuroscience, 154, 12181226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andersen, S. L. (2005). Stimulants and the developing brain. Trends in Pharmacological Sciences, 26, 237243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andersen, S. L., & Teicher, M. H. (2004). Delayed effects of early stress on hippocampal development. Neuropsychopharmacology, 29, 19881993.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andersen, S. L., & Teicher, M. H. (2008). Stress, sensitive periods and maturational events in adolescent depression. Trends in Neuroscience, 31, 183191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andersen, S. L., & Teicher, M. H. (2009). Desperately driven and no brakes: Developmental stress exposure and subsequent risk for substance abuse. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 33, 516524.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andersen, S. L., & Thompson, B. S. (2011). Reply to Abramowitz et al.: Animal Models of OCD. Biological Psychiatry, 69, e31e32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andersen, S. L., Tomada, A., Vincow, E. S., Valente, E., Polcari, A. & Teicher, M. H. (2008). Preliminary evidence for sensitive periods in the effect of childhood sexual abuse on regional brain development. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 20, 292301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anisman, H., Zaharia, M. D., Meaney, M. J., & Merali, Z. (1998). Do early-life events permanently alter behavioral and hormonal responses to stressors? International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 16, 149164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arakawa, H. (2007). Ontogeny of sex differences in defensive burying behavior in rats: Effect of social isolation. Aggression and Behavior, 33, 3847.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bangasser, D. A., & Valentino, R. J. (2012). Sex differences in molecular and cellular substrates of stress. Cellular and Molecular Neurobiology, 32, 709723.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barreau, F., Ferrier, L., Fioramonti, J., & Bueno, L. (2007). New insights in the etiology and pathophysiology of irritable bowel syndrome: Contribution of neonatal stress models. Pediatric Research, 62, 240245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beardslee, W. R., Gladstone, T. R., & O'Connor, E. E. (2012). Developmental risk of depression: Experience matters. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Clinics North America, 21, 261278, vii.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beitchman, J. H., Zucker, K. J., Hood, J. E., daCosta, G. A., Akman, D., & Cassavia, E. (1992). A review of the long-term effects of child sexual abuse. Child Abuse and Neglect, 16, 101118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernardi, J. R., Ferreira, C. F., Senter, G., Krolow, R., de Aguiar, B. W., Portella, A. K., et al. (2013). Early life stress interacts with the diet deficiency of omega-3 fatty acids during the life course increasing the metabolic vulnerability in adult rats. PLOS ONE, 8, e62031.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bevans, K., Cerbone, A., & Overstreet, S. (2008). Relations between recurrent trauma exposure and recent life stress and salivary cortisol among children. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 257272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blumberg, M. S., Deaver, K., & Kirby, R. F. (1999). Leptin disinhibits nonshivering thermogenesis in infants after maternal separation. American Journal of Physiology, 276, R606R610.Google ScholarPubMed
Bourke, C. H., & Neigh, G. N. (2011). Behavioral effects of chronic adolescent stress are sustained and sexually dimorphic. Hormones and Behavior, 60, 112120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowden, D. M., & McKinney, W. T. (1972). Behavioral effects of peer separation, isolation, and reunion on adolescent male rhesus monkeys. Developmental Psychobiology, 5, 353362.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Branchi, I., & Alleva, E. (2006). Communal nesting, an early social enrichment, increases the adult anxiety-like response and shapes the role of social context in modulating the emotional behavior. Behavioral Brain Research, 172, 299306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bremner, J. D., Randall, P., Vermetten, E., Staib, L., Bronen, R. A., Mazure, C., et al. (1997). Magnetic resonance imaging-based measurement of hippocampal volume in posttraumatic stress disorder related to childhood physical and sexual abuse—A preliminary report. Biological Psychiatry, 41, 2332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brenes, J. C., Padilla, M., & Fornaguera, J. (2009). A detailed analysis of open-field habituation and behavioral and neurochemical antidepressant-like effects in postweaning enriched rats. Behavioral Brain Research, 197, 125137.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brenhouse, H. C., & Andersen, S. L. (2011). Developmental trajectories during adolescence in males and females: A cross-species understanding of underlying brain changes. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 16871703.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brenhouse, H. C., Sonntag, K. C., & Andersen, S. L. (2008). Transient D1 dopamine receptor expression on prefrontal cortex projection neurons: Relationship to enhanced motivational salience of drug cues in adolescence. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 23752382.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brent, D. A. (2009). The treatment of SSRI-resistant depression in adolescents (TORDIA): In search of the best next step. Depression and Anxiety, 26, 871874.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Briere, J. (1992). Medical symptoms, health risk, and history of childhood sexual abuse. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 67, 603604.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, D. W., Anda, R. F., Edwards, V. J., Felitti, V. J., Dube, S. R., & Giles, W. H. (2007). Adverse childhood experiences and childhood autobiographical memory disturbance. Child Abuse and Neglect, 31, 961969.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brunelli, S. A., Shindledecker, R. D., & Hofer, M. A. (1989). Early experience and maternal behavior in rats. Developmental Psychobiology, 22, 295314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burdge, G. C., Lillycrop, K. A., & Jackson, A. A. (2009). Nutrition in early life, and risk of cancer and metabolic disease: Alternative endings in an epigenetic tale? British Journal of Nutrition, 101, 619630.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carrion, V. G., & Wong, S. S. (2012). Can traumatic stress alter the brain? Understanding the implications of early trauma on brain development and learning. Journal of Adolescent Health, 51, S23S28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castellanos, F. X., & Tannock, R. (2002). Neuroscience of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: The search for endophenotypes. Nature Review of Neuroscience, 3, 617628.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Champagne, F. A. (2013). Epigenetics and developmental plasticity across species. Developmental Psychobiology, 55, 3341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Gunnar, M. R. (2008). Integrating biological measures into the design and evaluation of preventive interventions. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 737743.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (2002). A developmental psychopathology perspective on adolescence. Journal of Consultation and Clinical Psychology, 70, 620.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., Gunnar, M. R., & Toth, S. L. (2010). The differential impacts of early physical and sexual abuse and internalizing problems on daytime cortisol rhythm in school-aged children. Child Development, 81, 252269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (1995). A developmental psychopathology perspective on child abuse and neglect. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 541565.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coccurello, R., Bielawski, A., Zelek-Molik, A., Vetulani, J., Kowalska, M., D'Amato, F. R. et al. (2014). Brief maternal separation affects brain alpha1-adrenoceptors and apoptotic signaling in adult mice. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 48, 161169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cochran, R. F. (2000). Men and depression: Clinical and empirical perspectives. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Copeland, W. E., Keeler, G., Angold, A., & Costello, E. J. (2007). Traumatic events and posttraumatic stress in childhood. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64, 577584.Google ScholarPubMed
Cressman, V. L., Balaban, J., Steinfeld, S., Shemyakin, A., Graham, P., Parisot, N. et al. (2010). Prefrontal cortical inputs to the basal amygdala undergo pruning during late adolescence in the rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 518, 26932709.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cromheeke, S., Herpoel, L. A., & Mueller, S. C. (in press). Childhood abuse is related to working memory impairment for positive emotion in female university students. Child Maltreatreatment,Google Scholar
Cruz, F. C., Marin, M. T., Leao, R. M., & Planeta, C. S. (2012). Behavioral and neuroendocrine effects of the exposure to chronic restraint or variable stress in early adolescent rats. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 30, 1923.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cunningham, M. G., Bhattacharyya, S., & Benes, F. M. (2002). Amygdalo–cortical sprouting continues into early adulthood: Implications for the development of normal and abnormal function during adolescence. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 453, 116130.Google ScholarPubMed
Cyranowski, J. M., Frank, E., Young, E., & Shear, M. K. (2000). Adolescent onset of the gender difference in lifetime rates of major depression: A theoretical model. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 2127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dalla, C., Edgecomb, C., Whetstone, A. S., & Shors, T. J. (2007). Females do not express learned helplessness like males do. Neuropsychopharmacology, 3, 15591569.Google Scholar
Dalla, C., Pitychoutis, P. M., Kokras, N., & Papadopoulou-Daifoti, Z. (2011). Sex differences in response to stress and expression of depressive-like behaviours in the rat. Current Topics in Behavior Neuroscience, 8, 97118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Da Silva, N. L., Ferreira, V. M., Carobrez Ade, P., & Morato, G. S. (1996). Individual housing from rearing modifies the performance of young rats on the elevated plus-maze apparatus. Physiology & Behavior, 60, 13911396.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeBellis, M. D., & Keshavan, M. S. (2003). Sex differences in brain maturation in maltreatment-related pediatric posttraumatic stress disorder. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 27, 103117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeBellis, M. D., Keshavan, M. S., Clark, D. B., Casey, B. J., Gledd, J. N., Boring, A. M., et al. (1999). A.E. Bennett Research Award: Developmental traumatology: Part II. Brain development. Biological Psychiatry, 45, 12711284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeBellis, M. D., Narasimhan, A., Thatcher, D. L., Keshavan, M. S., Soloff, P., & Clark, D. B. (2005). Prefrontal cortex, thalamus, and cerebellar volumes in adolescents and young adults with adolescent-onset alcohol use disorders and comorbid mental disorders. Alcohol and Clinical Experimental Research, 29, 15901600.Google Scholar
Delahaye, F., Breton, C., Risold, P. Y., Enache, M., Dutriez-Casteloot, I., Laborie, C., et al. (2008). Maternal perinatal undernutrition drastically reduces postnatal leptin surge and affects the development of arcuate nucleus proopiomelanocortin neurons in neonatal male rat pups. Endocrinology, 149, 470475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delaunay, F., Khan, A., Cintra, A., Davani, B., Ling, Z. C., Andersson, A., et al. (1997). Pancreatic beta cells are important targets for the diabetogenic effects of glucocorticoids. Journal of Clinical Investigation, 100, 20942098.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denenberg, V. H., Garbanati, J., Sherman, D. A., Yutzey, D. A., & Kaplan, R. (1978). Infantile stimulation induces brain lateralization in rats. Science, 201, 11501152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Der-Avakian, A., & Markou, A. (2010). Neonatal maternal separation exacerbates the reward-enhancing effect of acute amphetamine administration and the anhedonic effect of repeated social defeat in adult rats. Neuroscience, 170, 11891198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Detke, M. J., Rickels, M., & Lucki, I. (1995). Active behaviors in the rat forced swimming test differentially produced by serotonergic and noradrenergic antidepressants. Psychopharmacology, 121, 6672.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doom, J. R., Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., & Dackis, M. N. (2013). Child maltreatment and gender interactions as predictors of differential neuroendocrine profiles. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38, 14421454.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dube, S. R., Anda, R. F., Felitti, V. J., Chapman, D. P., Williamson, D. F., & Giles, W. H. (2001). Childhood abuse, household dysfunction, and the risk of attempted suicide throughout the life span: Findings from the Adverse Childhood Experiences Study. Journal of the American Medical Association, 286, 30893096.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duggal, S., Carlson, E. A., Sroufe, L. A., & Egeland, B. (2001). Depressive symptomatology in childhood and adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 143164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dugovic, C., Maccari, S., Weibel, L., Turek, F. W., & Van Reeth, O. (1999). High corticosterone levels in prenatally stressed rats predict persistent paradoxical sleep alterations. Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 86568664.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, E. C., McLaughlin, K. A., Slopen, N., Rosand, J., & Smoller, J. W. (2013). Developmental timing of child maltreatment and symptoms of depression and suicidal ideation in young adulthood: Results from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health. Depression and Anxiety, 30, 955964.Google ScholarPubMed
Eiland, L., & Romeo, R. D. (2013). Stress and the developing adolescent brain. Neuroscience, 249, 162171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feiring, C., Taska, L., & Lewis, M. (1999). Age and gender differences in children's and adolescents' adaptation to sexual abuse. Child Abuse and Neglect, 23, 115128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Felitti, V. J. (1991). Long-term medical consequences of incest, rape, and molestation. South Medical Journal, 84, 328331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Felitti, V. J., Anda, R. F., Nordenberg, D., Williamson, D. F., Spitz, A. M., Edwards, V., et al. (1998). Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults: The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. American Journal of Preventative Medicine, 14, 245258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feng, M., Sheng, G., Li, Z., Wang, J., Ren, K., Jin, X., et al. (2013). Postnatal maternal separation enhances tonic GABA current of cortical layer 5 pyramidal neurons in juvenile rats and promotes genesis of GABAergic neurons in neocortical molecular layer and subventricular zone in adult rats. Behavioral Brain Research, 260, 7482.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fergusson, D. M., McLeod, G. F., & Horwood, L. J. (2013). Childhood sexual abuse and adult developmental outcomes: Findings from a 30-year longitudinal study in New Zealand. Child Abuse and Neglect, 37, 664674.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
File, S. E. (1978). Exploration, distraction, and habituation in rats reared in isolation. Developmental Psychobiology, 11, 7381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fishbein, D. (2000). The importance of neurobiological research to the prevention of psychopathology. Prevention Science, 1, 89106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frankola, K. A., Flora, A. L., Torres, A. K., Grissom, E. M., Overstreet, S., & Dohanich, G. P. (2010). Effects of early rearing conditions on cognitive performance in prepubescent male and female rats. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 94, 9199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freund, N., Thompson, B. S., Denormandie, J., Vaccarro, K., & Andersen, S. L. (2013). Windows of vulnerability: Maternal separation, age, and fluoxetine on adolescent depressive-like behavior in rats. Neuroscience, 249, 8897.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, E., Berman, M., & Overstreet, D. (2006). Swim test immobility in a genetic rat model of depression is modified by maternal environment: A cross-foster study. Developmental Psychobiology, 48, 169177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuller-Thomson, E., Baker, T. M., & Brennenstuhl, S. (2012). Evidence supporting an independent association between childhood physical abuse and lifetime suicidal ideation. Suicide and Life Threatening Behaivor, 42, 279291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giedd, J. N., Castellanos, F. X., Rajapakse, J. C., Vaituzis, A. C., & Rapoport, J. L. (1997). Sexual dimorphism of the developing human brain. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 21, 11851201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilmer, W. S., & McKinney, W. T. (2003). Early experience and depressive disorders: Human and non-human primate studies. Journal of Affective Disorders, 75, 97113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gladstone, G. L., Parker, G. B., Mitchell, P. B., Malhi, G. S., Wilhelm, K., & Austin, M. P. (2004). Implications of childhood trauma for depressed women: An analysis of pathways from childhood sexual abuse to deliberate self-harm and revictimization. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 14171425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, G. S., Quas, J. A., & Ogle, C. M. (2010). Child maltreatment and memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 325351.Google ScholarPubMed
Gorisch, J., & Schwarting, R. K. (2006). Wistar rats with high versus low rearing activity differ in radial maze performance. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 86, 175187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenough, W. T., Black, J. E., & Wallace, C. S. (1987). Experience and brain development. Child Development, 58, 539559.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gustafson, T. B., & Sarwer, D. B. (2004). Childhood sexual abuse and obesity. Obesity Reviews, 5, 129135.Google ScholarPubMed
Hajszan, T., Szigeti-Buck, K., Sallam, N. L., Bober, J., Parducz, A., Maclusky, N. J., et al. (2010). Effects of estradiol on learned helplessness and associated remodeling of hippocampal spine synapses in female rats. Biological Psychiatry, 67, 168174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, F. S., Huang, S., Fong, G. W., Pert, A., & Linnoila, M. (1998). Effects of isolation-rearing on locomotion, anxiety and responses to ethanol in Fawn Hooded and Wistar rats. Psychopharmacology, 139, 203209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, F. S., Humby, T., Wilkinson, L. S., & Robbins, T. W. (1997). The effects of isolation-rearing on sucrose consumption in rats. Physiology & Behavior, 62, 291297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haller, J., & Kruk, M. R. (2006). Normal and abnormal aggression: Human disorders and novel laboratory models. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 292303.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harlow, H. F., & Harlow, M. K. (1965). The effect of rearing conditions on behavior. International Journal of Psychiatry, 1, 4351.Google ScholarPubMed
Harmer, C. J., & Phillips, G. D. (1998). Isolation rearing enhances acquisition in a conditioned inhibition paradigm. Physiology & Behavior, 65, 525533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heim, C., Nater, U. M., Maloney, E., Boneva, R., Jones, J. F., & Reeves, W. C. (2009). Childhood trauma and risk for chronic fatigue syndrome: Association with neuroendocrine dysfunction. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66, 7280.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heim, C., Wagner, D., Maloney, E., Papanicolaou, D. A., Solomon, L., Jones, J. F., et al. (2006). Early adverse experience and risk for chronic fatigue syndrome: Results from a population-based study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63, 12581266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herba, C. M., Tremblay, R. E., Boivin, M., Liu, X., Mongeau, C., Seguin, J. R., et al. (2013). Maternal depressive symptoms and children's emotional problems: Can early child care help children of depressed mothers? JAMA Psychiatry, 70, 830838.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinde, R. A., Spencer-Booth, Y., & Bruce, M. (1966). Effects of 6-day maternal deprivation on rhesus monkey infants. Nature, 210, 10211023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofer, M. A. (1975). Studies on how early maternal deprivation produces behavioral change in young rats. Psychosomatic Medicine, 37, 245264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hong, S., Flashner, B., Chiu, M., ver Hoeve, E., Luz, S., & Bhatnagar, S. (2012). Social isolation in adolescence alters behaviors in the forced swim and sucrose preference tests in female but not in male rats. Physiology & Behavior, 105, 269275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1970). The period of susceptibility to the physiological effects of unilateral eye closure in kittens. Journal of Physiology, 206, 419436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1998). Early exploration of the visual cortex. Neuron, 20, 401412.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hubel, D. H., Wiesel, T. N., & LeVay, S. (1977). Plasticity of ocular dominance columns in monkey striate cortex. Philosophical Transcripts of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 278, 377409.Google ScholarPubMed
Huot, R. L., Plotsky, P. M., Lenox, R. H., & McNamara, R. K. (2002). Neonatal maternal separation reduces hippocampal mossy fiber density in adult Long Evans rats. Brain Research, 950, 5263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackowski, A., Perera, T. D., Abdallah, C. G., Garrido, G., Tang, C. Y., Martinez, J., et al. (2011). Early-life stress, corpus callosum development, hippocampal volumetrics, and anxious behavior in male nonhuman primates. Psychiatry Research, 192, 3744.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jankord, R., Solomon, M. B., Albertz, J., Flak, J. N., Zhang, R., & Herman, J. P. (2011). Stress vulnerability during adolescent development in rats. Endocrinology, 152, 629638.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Juraska, J. M., Henderson, C., & Muller, J. (1984). Differential rearing experience, gender, and radial maze performance. Developmental Psychobiology, 17, 209215.Google ScholarPubMed
Kaplow, J. B., & Widom, C. S. (2007). Age of onset of child maltreatment predicts long-term mental health outcomes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 176187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, D., Banerji, M. A., Shorman, I., Smith, E. L., Coplan, J. D., Rosenblum, L. A., et al. (2007). Early-life stress and the development of obesity and insulin resistance in juvenile bonnet macaques. Diabetes, 56, 13821386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, J., Plotsky, P. M., Nemeroff, C. B., & Charney, D. S. (2000). Effects of early adverse experiences on brain structure and function: Clinical implications. Biological Psychiatry, 48, 778790.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Bulik, C. M., Silberg, J., Hettema, J. M., Myers, J., & Prescott, C. A. (2000). Childhood sexual abuse and adult psychiatric and substance use disorders in women: An epidemiological and cotwin control analysis. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 953959.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., McGonagle, K. A., Zhao, S., Nelson, C. B., Hughes, M., Eshleman, S., et al. (1994). Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States: Results from the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 819.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kinkead, R., Montandon, G., Bairam, A., Lajeunesse, Y., & Horner, R. (2009). Neonatal maternal separation disrupts regulation of sleep and breathing in adult male rats. Sleep, 32, 16111620.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiyono, S., Seo, M. L., & Shibagaki, M. (1981). Effects of rearing environments upon sleep–waking parameters in rats. Physiology & Behavior, 26, 391394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klomek, A. B., Kleinman, M., Altschuler, E., Marrocco, F., Amakawa, L., & Gould, M. S. (2013). Suicidal adolescents' experiences with bullying perpetration and victimization during high school as risk factors for later depression and suicidality. Journal of Adolescent Health, 53, S37S42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koenen, K. C., & Widom, C. S. (2009). A prospective study of sex differences in the lifetime risk of posttraumatic stress disorder among abused and neglected children grown up. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 22, 566574.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kornetsky, C., & Esposito, R. U. (1979). Euphorigenic drugs: Effects on the reward pathways of the brain. Federation Proceedings, 38, 24732476.Google ScholarPubMed
Krakow, B., Melendrez, D., Johnston, L., Warner, T. D., Clark, J. O., Pacheco, M., et al. (2002). Sleep-disordered breathing, psychiatric distress, and quality of life impairment in sexual assault survivors. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disorders, 190, 442452.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kugler, B. B., Bloom, M., Kaercher, L. B., Truax, T. V., & Storch, E. A. (2012). Somatic symptoms in traumatized children and adolescents. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 43, 661673.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhn, C. M., Butler, S. R., & Schanberg, S. M. (1978). Selective depression of serum growth hormone during maternal deprivation in rat pups. Science, 201, 10341036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhn, C. M., Pauk, J., & Schanberg, S. M. (1990). Endocrine responses to mother–infant separation in developing rats. Developmental Psychobiology, 23, 395410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kumpulainen, K., Rasanen, E., Henttonen, I., Almqvist, F., Kresanov, K., Linna, S. L., et al. (1998). Bullying and psychiatric symptoms among elementary school-age children. Child Abuse and Neglect, 22, 705717.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kundakovic, M., Lim, S., Gudsnuk, K., & Champagne, F. A. (2013). Sex-specific and strain-dependent effects of early life adversity on behavioral and epigenetic outcomes. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 4, 78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuppermann, B. D., & Kasamatsu, T. (1984). Enhanced binocular interaction in the visual cortex of normal kittens subjected to intracortical norepinephrine perfusion. Brain Research, 302, 9199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuramochi, M., & Nakamura, S. (2009). Effects of postnatal isolation rearing and antidepressant treatment on the density of serotonergic and noradrenergic axons and depressive behavior in rats. Neuroscience, 163, 448455.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lambillotte, C., Gilon, P., & Henquin, J. C. (1997). Direct glucocorticoid inhibition of insulin secretion: An in vitro study of dexamethasone effects in mouse islets. Journal of Clinical Investigation, 99, 414423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laudenslager, M. L., Held, P. E., Boccia, M. L., Reite, M. L., & Cohen, J. J. (1990). Behavioral and immunological consequences of brief mother–infant separation: A species comparison. Developmental Psychobiology, 23, 247264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, H. J., Kim, J. W., Yim, S. V., Kim, M. J., Kim, S. A., Kim, Y. J., et al. (2001). Fluoxetine enhances cell proliferation and prevents apoptosis in dentate gyrus of maternally separated rats. Molecular Psychiatry, 6, 725728.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lehmann, J., Pryce, C. R., Bettschen, D., & Feldon, J. (1999). The maternal separation paradigm and adult emotionality and cognition in male and female Wistar rats. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 64, 705715.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leussis, M. P., & Andersen, S. L. (2008). Is adolescence a sensitive period for depression? Behavioral and neuroanatomical findings from a social stress model. Synapse, 62, 2230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leussis, M. P., Freund, N., Brenhouse, H. C., Thompson, B. S., & Andersen, S. L. (2012). Depressive-like behavior in adolescents after maternal separation: Sex differences, controllability, and GABA. Developmental Neuroscience, 34, 210217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leventopoulos, M., Russig, H., Feldon, J., Pryce, C. R., & Opacka-Juffry, J. (2009). Early deprivation leads to long-term reductions in motivation for reward and 5-HT1A binding and both effects are reversed by fluoxetine. Neuropharmacology, 56, 692701.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levine, S. (1967). Maternal and environmental influences on the adrenocortical response to stress in weanling rats. Science, 156, 258260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levine, S., Johnson, D. F., & Gonzalez, C. A. (1985). Behavioral and hormonal responses to separation in infant rhesus monkeys and mothers. Behavioral Neuroscience, 99, 399410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Litvin, Y., Tovote, P., Pentkowski, N. S., Zeyda, T., King, L. B., Vasconcellos, A. J., et al. (2010). Maternal separation modulates short-term behavioral and physiological indices of the stress response. Hormones and Behavior, 58, 241249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, D., Diorio, J., Tannenbaum, B., Caldji, C., Francis, D., Freedman, A., et al. (1997). Maternal care, hippocampal glucocorticoid receptors, and hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal responses to stress. Science, 277, 16591662.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lukkes, J., Vuong, S., Scholl, J., Oliver, H., & Forster, G. (2009). Corticotropin-releasing factor receptor antagonism within the dorsal raphe nucleus reduces social anxiety-like behavior after early-life social isolation. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 99559960.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lukkes, J. L., Watt, M. J., Lowry, C. A., & Forster, G. L. (2009). Consequences of post-weaning social isolation on anxiety behavior and related neural circuits in rodents. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 3, 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacMillan, H. L., Fleming, J. E., Streiner, D. L., Lin, E., Boyle, M. H., Jamieson, E., et al. (2001). Childhood abuse and lifetime psychopathology in a community sample. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 18781883.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macri, S., Laviola, G., Leussis, M. P., & Andersen, S. L. (2009). Abnormal behavioral and neurotrophic development in the younger sibling receiving less maternal care in a communal nursing paradigm in rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 35, 392402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macri, S., Mason, G. J., & Wurbel, H. (2004). Dissociation in the effects of neonatal maternal separations on maternal care and the offspring's HPA and fear responses in rats. European Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 10171024.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macri, S., Zoratto, F., & Laviola, G. (2011). Early-stress regulates resilience, vulnerability and experimental validity in laboratory rodents through mother-offspring hormonal transfer. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 15341543.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maestripieri, D. (2005). Early experience affects the intergenerational transmission of infant abuse in rhesus monkeys. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102, 97269729.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maier, S. F., & Watkins, L. R. (2005). Stressor controllability and learned helplessness: The roles of the dorsal raphe nucleus, serotonin, and corticotropin-releasing factor. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 29, 829841.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marais, L., van Rensburg, S. J., van Zyl, J. M., Stein, D. J., & Daniels, W. M. (2008). Maternal separation of rat pups increases the risk of developing depressive-like behavior after subsequent chronic stress by altering corticosterone and neurotrophin levels in the hippocampus. Neuroscience Research, 61, 106112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcus, S. M., Young, E. A., Kerber, K. B., Kornstein, S., Farabaugh, A. H., Mitchell, J., et al. (2005). Gender differences in depression: Findings from the STAR*D study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 87, 141150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matthews, K., Hall, F. S., Wilkinson, L. S., & Robbins, T. W. (1996). Retarded acquisition and reduced expression of conditioned locomotor activity in adult rats following repeated early maternal separation: Effects of prefeeding, d-amphetamine, dopamine antagonists and clonidine. Psychopharmacology, 126, 7584.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matthews, K., & Robbins, T. W. (2003). Early experience as a determinant of adult behavioural responses to reward: The effects of repeated maternal separation in the rat. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 27, 4555.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matthews, K., Wilkinson, L. S., & Robbins, T. W. (1996). Repeated maternal separation of preweanling rats attenuates behavioral responses to primary and conditioned incentives in adulthood. Physiology & Behavior, 59, 99107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McEwen, B. S. (2000). Allostasis and allostatic load: Implications for neuropsychopharmacology. Neuropsychopharmacology, 22, 108124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mehta, N. S., Wang, L., & Redei, E. E. (2013). Sex differences in depressive, anxious behaviors and hippocampal transcript levels in a genetic rat model. Genes, Brain, and Behavior, 12, 695704.Google Scholar
Michaels, C. C., Easterling, K. W., & Holtzman, S. G. (2007). Maternal separation alters ICSS responding in adult male and female rats, but morphine and naltrexone have little affect on that behavior. Brain Research Bulletin, 73, 310318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitchell, J., McCauley, E., Burke, P. M., & Moss, S. J. (1988). Phenomenology of depression in children and adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 27, 1220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moriceau, S., Wilson, D. A., Levine, S., & Sullivan, R. M. (2006). Dual circuitry for odor-shock conditioning during infancy: Corticosterone switches between fear and attraction via amygdala. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 67376748.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morielli, A., Ladan, S., Ducharme, F. M., & Brouillette, R. T. (1996). Can sleep and wakefulness be distinguished in children by cardiorespiratory and videotape recordings? Chest, 109, 680687.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mourlon, V., Baudin, A., Blanc, O., Lauber, A., Giros, B., Naudon, L., et al. (2010). Maternal deprivation induces depressive-like behaviours only in female rats. Behavioral Brain Research, 213, 278287.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mrdalj, J., Pallesen, S., Milde, A. M., Jellestad, F. K., Murison, R., Ursin, R., et al. (2013). Early and later life stress alters brain activity and sleep in rats. PLOS ONE, 8, e69923.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Navalta, C. P., Polcari, A., Webster, D. M., Boghossian, A., & Teicher, M. H. (2006). Effects of childhood sexual abuse on neuropsychological and cognitive function in college women. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 18, 4553.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, E. C., Heath, A. C., Madden, P. A., Cooper, M. L., Dinwiddie, S. H., Bucholz, K. K., et al. (2002). Association between self-reported childhood sexual abuse and adverse psychosocial outcomes: Results from a twin study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 139145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, E. E., Herman, K. N., Barrett, C. E., Noble, P. L., Wojteczko, K., Chisholm, K., et al. (2009). Adverse rearing experiences enhance responding to both aversive and rewarding stimuli in juvenile rhesus monkeys. Biological Psychiatry, 66, 702704.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noll, J. G., Trickett, P. K., Susman, E. J., & Putnam, F. W. (2006). Sleep disturbances and childhood sexual abuse. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 31, 469480.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olton, D. S., Johnson, C. T., & Howard, E. (1974). Impairment of conditioned active avoidance in adult rats given corticosterone in infancy. Developmental Psychobiology, 8, 5561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Overstreet, D. H. (2012). Modeling depression in animal models. Methods in Molecular Biology, 829, 125144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Papaioannou, A., Gerozissis, K., Prokopiou, A., Bolaris, S., & Stylianopoulou, F. (2002). Sex differences in the effects of neonatal handling on the animal's response to stress and the vulnerability for depressive behaviour. Behavioral Brain Research, 129, 131139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paternain, L., Martisova, E., Milagro, F. I., Ramirez, M. J., Martinez, J. A., & Campion, J. (2012). Postnatal maternal separation modifies the response to an obesogenic diet in adulthood in rats. Disease Models and Mechansims, 5, 691697.Google Scholar
Paul, I. A., English, J. A., & Halaris, A. (2000). Sucrose and quinine intake by maternally-deprived and control rhesus monkeys. Behavioral Brain Research, 112, 127134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pervanidou, P., & Chrousos, G. P. (2011). Stress and obesity/metabolic syndrome in childhood and adolescence. International Journal of Pediatric Obesity, 6 (Suppl. 1), 2128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plescia, F., Marino, R. A., Navarra, M., Gambino, G., Brancato, A., Sardo, P., et al. (2013). Early handling effect on female rat spatial and non-spatial learning and memory. Behavioural Processes. Advance online publication.Google Scholar
Plotsky, P. M., & Meaney, M. J. (1993). Early, postnatal experience alters hypothalamic corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) mRNA, median eminence CRF content and stress-induced release in adult rats. Brain Research: Molecular Brain Research, 18, 195200.Google ScholarPubMed
Plotsky, P. M., Thrivikraman, K. V., Nemeroff, C. B., Caldji, C., Sharma, S., & Meaney, M. J. (2005). Long-term consequences of neonatal rearing on central corticotropin-releasing factor systems in adult male rat offspring. Neuropsychopharmacology, 30, 21922204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollack, W. (1998). Mourning, melancholia and masculinity: Recognizing and treating depression in men. New psychotherapy for men (pp. 147166). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Quan, M. N., Tian, Y. T., Xu, K. H., Zhang, T., & Yang, Z. (2010). Postweaning social isolation influences spatial cognition, prefrontal cortical synaptic plasticity and hippocampal potassium ion channels in Wistar rats. Neuroscience, 169, 214222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raineki, C., Cortes, M. R., Belnoue, L., & Sullivan, R. M. (2012). Effects of early-life abuse differ across development: Infant social behavior deficits are followed by adolescent depressive-like behaviors mediated by the amygdala. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 77587765.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rao, H., Betancourt, L., Giannetta, J. M., Brodsky, N. L., Korczykowski, M., Avants, B. B., et al. (2010). Early parental care is important for hippocampal maturation: Evidence from brain morphology in humans. NeuroImage, 49, 11441150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reus, G. Z., Stringari, R. B., Ribeiro, K. F., Cipriano, A. L., Panizzutti, B. S., Stertz, L., et al. (2011). Maternal deprivation induces depressive-like behaviour and alters neurotrophin levels in the rat brain. Neurochemical Research, 36, 460466.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rimsza, M. E., Berg, R. A., & Locke, C. (1988). Sexual abuse: Somatic and emotional reactions. Child Abuse and Neglect, 12, 201208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robbins, T. W. (2005). Controlling stress: How the brain protects itself from depression. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 261262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenblum, L. A., & Paully, G. S. (1984). The effects of varying environmental demands on maternal and infant behavior. Child Development, 55, 305314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruedi-Bettschen, D., Zhang, W., Russig, H., Ferger, B., Weston, A., Pedersen, E. M., et al. (2006). Early deprivation leads to altered behavioural, autonomic and endocrine responses to environmental challenge in adult Fischer rats. European Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 28792893.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryu, V., Yoo, S. B., Kang, D. W., Lee, J. H., & Jahng, J. W. (2009). Post-weaning isolation promotes food intake and body weight gain in rats that experienced neonatal maternal separation. Brain Research, 1295, 127134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (1977). Isolation-rearing enhances tail pinch-induced oral behavior in rats. Physiology & Behavior, 18, 5358.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salzmann, C., Otis, M., Long, H., Roberge, C., Gallo-Payet, N., & Walker, C. D. (2004). Inhibition of steroidogenic response to adrenocorticotropin by leptin: Implications for the adrenal response to maternal separation in neonatal rats. Endocrinology, 145, 18101822.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schoedl, A. F., Costa, M. C., Mari, J. J., Mello, M. F., Tyrka, A. R., Carpenter, L. L., et al. (2010). The clinical correlates of reported childhood sexual abuse: An association between age at trauma onset and severity of depression and PTSD in adults. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 19, 156170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schuck, A. M., & Widom, C. S. (2001). Childhood victimization and alcohol symptoms in females: Causal inferences and hypothesized mediators. Child Abuse and Neglect, 25, 10691092.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smotherman, W. P., Wiener, S. G., Mendoza, S. P., & Levine, S. (1977). Maternal pituitary–adrenal responsiveness as a function of differential treatment of rat pups. Developmental Psychobiology, 10, 113122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solas, M., Aisa, B., Mugueta, M. C., Del Rio, J., Tordera, R. M., & Ramirez, M. J. (2010). Interactions between age, stress and insulin on cognition: Implications for Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35, 16641673.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spinelli, S., Chefer, S., Suomi, S. J., Higley, J. D., Barr, C. S., & Stein, E. (2009). Early-life stress induces long-term morphologic changes in primate brain. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66, 658665.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanis, J. T., & Andersen, S. (in press). Reducing substance use during adolescence: A translational framework for prevention. Psychopharmacology.Google Scholar
Sterley, T. L., Howells, F. M., & Russell, V. A. (2011). Effects of early life trauma are dependent on genetic predisposition: A rat study. Behavior and Brain Function, 7, 11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevenson, C. W., Meredith, J. P., Spicer, C. H., Mason, R., & Marsden, C. A. (2009). Early life programming of innate fear and fear learning in adult female rats. Behavioral Brain Research, 198, 5157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Struber, N., Struber, D., & Roth, G. (2013). Impact of early adversity on glucocorticoid regulation and later mental disorders. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 38C, 1737.Google Scholar
Studts, C. R., & van Zyl, M. A. (2013). Identification of developmentally appropriate screening items for disruptive behavior problems in preschoolers. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41, 851863.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sundstrom, J. M., Hall, F. S., Stellar, J. R., & Waugh, E. J. (2002). Effects of isolation-rearing on intracranial self-stimulation reward of the lateral hypothalamus: Baseline assessment and drug challenges. Life Sciences, 70, 27992810.Google ScholarPubMed
Suomi, S. J., Delizio, R., & Harlow, H. F. (1976). Social rehabilitation of separation-induced depressive disorders in monkeys. American Journal of Psychiatry, 133, 12791285.Google ScholarPubMed
Suomi, S. J., Harlow, H. F., & Domek, C. J. (1970). Effect of repetitive infant–infant separation of young monkeys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 76, 161172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taft, A. J., Small, R., Hegarty, K. L., Watson, L. F., Gold, L., & Lumley, J. A. (2011). Mothers' AdvocateS In the Community (MOSAIC)—Non-professional mentor support to reduce intimate partner violence and depression in mothers: A cluster randomised trial in primary care. BMC Public Health, 11, 178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tang, A. C., Reeb-Sutherland, B. C., Romeo, R. D., & McEwen, B. S. (2013). On the causes of early life experience effects: Evaluating the role of mom. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, S0091–S0302.Google Scholar
Taylor, R. R., & Jason, L. A. (2002). Chronic fatigue, abuse-related traumatization, and psychiatric disorders in a community-based sample. Society of Scientific Medicine, 55, 247256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, S. B., Taylor, A. R., & Koenig, J. I. (2013). The interaction of disrupted Type II Neuregulin 1 and chronic adolescent stress on adult anxiety- and fear-related behaviors. Neuroscience, 249, 3142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teicher, M. H. (2002). Scars that won't heal: The neurobiology of child abuse. Scientific American, 286, 6875.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teicher, M. H., Dumont, N. L., Ito, Y., Vaituzis, C., Giedd, J. N., & Andersen, S. L. (2004). Childhood neglect is associated with reduced corpus callosum area. Biological Psychiatry, 56, 8085.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teicher, M. H., Samson, J. A., Polcari, A., & Andersen, S. L. (2009). Length of time between onset of childhood sexual abuse and emergence of depression in a young adult sample: A retrospective clinical report. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 70, 684691.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomoda, A., Sheu, Y. S., Rabi, K., Suzuki, H., Navalta, C. P., Polcari, A., et al. (2011). Exposure to parental verbal abuse is associated with increased gray matter volume in superior temporal gyrus. NeuroImage, 54(Suppl. 1), S280S286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomoda, A., Suzuki, H., Rabi, K., Sheu, Y. S., Polcari, A., & Teicher, M. H. (2009). Reduced prefrontal cortical gray matter volume in young adults exposed to harsh corporal punishment. NeuroImage, 47(Suppl. 2), T66T71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toth, S. L., & Cicchetti, D. (2013). A developmental psychopathology perspective on child maltreatment. Child Maltreatreatment, 18, 135139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trickett, P. K., Noll, J. G., Reiffman, A., & Putnam, F. W. (2001). Variants of intrafamilial sexual abuse experience: Implications for short- and long-term development. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 10011019.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tupler, L. A., & DeBellis, M. D. (2006). Segmented hippocampal volume in children and adolescents with posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 59, 523529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vamosi, M., Heitmann, B. L., & Kyvik, K. O. (2010). The relation between an adverse psychological and social environment in childhood and the development of adult obesity: A systematic literature review. Obesity Reviews, 11, 177184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Werff, S. J., Pannekoek, J. N., Veer, I. M., van Tol, M. J., Aleman, A., Veltman, D. J., et al. . (2013a). Resilience to childhood maltreatment is associated with increased resting-state functional connectivity of the salience network with the lingual gyrus. Child Abuse and Neglect, 37, 10211029.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Werff, S. J., Pannekoek, J. N., Veer, I. M., van Tol, M. J., Aleman, A., Veltman, D. J., et al. (2013b). Resting-state functional connectivity in adults with childhood emotional maltreatment. Psychological Medicine, 43, 18251836.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Veen, T., Wardenaar, K. J., Carlier, I. V., Spinhoven, P., Penninx, B. W. & Zitman, F. G. (2013). Are childhood and adult life adversities differentially associated with specific symptom dimensions of depression and anxiety? Testing the tripartite model. Journal of Affective Disorders, 146, 238245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ver Hoeve, E. S., Kelly, G., Luz, S., Ghanshani, S., & Bhatnagar, S. (2013). Short-term and long-term effects of repeated social defeat during adolescence or adulthood in female rats. Neuroscience, 249, 6373.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Volkmar, F. R., & Greenough, W. T. (1972). Rearing complexity affects branching of dendrites in the visual cortex of the rat. Science, 176, 14451447.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wachs, T. D., Georgieff, M., Cusick, S., & McEwen, B. S. (2013). Issues in the timing of integrated early interventions: Contributions from nutrition, neuroscience, and psychological research. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. Advance online publication.Google Scholar
Warren, W. G., & Ivinskis, A. (1973). Isolation-rearing effects on emotionality of hooded rats. Psychological Reports, 32, 10111014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weathington, J. M., Arnold, A. R., & Cooke, B. M. (2012). Juvenile social subjugation induces a sex-specific pattern of anxiety and depression-like behaviors in adult rats. Hormones and Behavior, 61, 9199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Welberg, L., Thrivikraman, K. V., & Plotsky, P. M. (2006). Combined pre- and postnatal environmental enrichment programs the HPA axis differentially in male and female rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 31, 553564.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Widom, C. S., DuMont, K., & Czaja, S. J. (2007). A prospective investigation of major depressive disorder and comorbidity in abused and neglected children grown up. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64, 4956.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wigger, A., & Neumann, I. D. (1999). Periodic maternal deprivation induces gender-dependent alterations in behavioral and neuroendocrine responses to emotional stress in adult rats. Physiology & Behavior, 66, 293302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, M. E., Bounar, S., Godfrey, J., Michopoulos, V., Higgins, M., & Sanchez, M. (2013). Social and emotional predictors of the tempo of puberty in female rhesus monkeys. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38, 6783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winterfeld, K. T., Teuchert-Noodt, G., & Dawirs, R. R. (1998). Social environment alters both ontogeny of dopamine innervation of the medial prefrontal cortex and maturation of working memory in gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus). Journal of Neuroscience Research, 52, 201209.3.0.CO;2-E>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolk, S., & Weissman, M. (1995). Women and depression: An update. American Psychiatric Press review of psychiatry (pp. 1422714259). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.Google Scholar
Woolley, C. S., Gould, E., Frankfurt, M., & McEwen, B. S. (1990). Naturally occurring fluctuation in dendritic spine density on adult hippocampal pyramidal neurons. Journal of Neuroscience, 10, 40354039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wright, I. K., Upton, N., & Marsden, C. A. (1991). Resocialisation of isolation-reared rats does not alter their anxiogenic profile on the elevated X-maze model of anxiety. Physiology & Behavior, 50, 11291132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, E., & Korszun, A. (2010). Sex, trauma, stress hormones and depression. Molecular Psychiatry, 15, 2328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed